References - An Introduction to Applied Cognitive Psychology - David Groome, Anthony Esgate, Michael W. Eysenck

An Introduction to Applied Cognitive Psychology - David Groome, Anthony Esgate, Michael W. Eysenck (2016)

References

Aarts, K. and Pourtois, G. (2012). Anxiety disrupts the evaluative component of performance monitoring: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 50, 1286-1296.

Abdul Razzak, R., Bagust, J., Docherty, S., Hasana, Z., Irshad, Y. and Rabiah, A. (2015). Menstrual phase influences gender differences in visual dependence: a study with a computerised rod and frame test. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(1), 80-88.

Ackerman, J.P., Riggins, T. and Black, M.M. (2010). A review of the effects of prenatal cocaine exposure among school-aged children. Pediatrics, 125(3), 554-565.

ACPO. (2004). Management of volume crime. Bramshill: National Centre for Policing Excellence.

Adam, E.K., Hawkley, L.C., Kudielka, B.M. and Cacioppo, J.T. (2006). Day-to-day dynamics of experience-cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(45), 17058-17063.

Adan, A., Archer, S.N., Hidalgo, M.P., Di Milia, L., Natale, V. and Randler, C. (2012). Circadian typology: a comprehensive review. Chronobiology International, 29(9), 1153-1175.

Ahmed, A., Khan, F., Ali, M., Haqnawaz, F., Hussain, A. and Azam, S.I. (2012). Effect of the menstrual cycle phase on post-operative pain perception and analgesic requirements. Acta Anaes thesiologica Scandinavica, 56(5), 629-635.

Ajzen, I. (2002). Residual effects of past behaviour on later behaviour: habituation and reasoned action perspectives. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 107-122.

Åkerstedt, T. (1995). Work hours, sleepiness and accidents: introduction and summary. Journal of Sleep Research, 4 (s2), 1-3.

Åkerstedt, T., Peters, B., Anund, A. and Kecklund, G. (2005). Impaired alertness and performance driving home from the night shift: a driving simulator study. Journal of Sleep Research, 14(1), 17-20.

Åkerstedt, T. and Wright, K.P. (2009). Sleep loss and fatigue in shift work and shift work disorder. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 4(2), 257-271.

Algan, O., Furedy, J.J., Demirgoren, Vincent, A. and Pogun, S. (1997). Effects of tobacco smoking and gender on interhemispheric cognitive function: per formance and confidence measures. Behavioural Pharmacology, 8, 416-428.

Allen, G.A., Mahler, W.A. and Estes, W.K. (1969). Effects of recall tests on long-term retention of paired associates. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 8, 463-470.

Allen, M.S., Jones, M., McCarthy, P.J., Sheehan-Mansfield, S. and Sheffield, D. (2013). Emotions correlate with perceived mental effort and concentration disruption in adult sport performance. European Journal of Sport Science, 13, 697-706.

Allen, P.A., Grabbe, J., McCarthy, A., Bush, A.H. and Wallace, B. (2008). The early bird does not get the worm: time-of-day effects on college students’ basic cognitive processing. The American Journal of Psychology, 121(4), 551-564.

Allport, A., Antonis, B. and Reynolds, P. (1972). On the division of attention: a disproof of the single channel hypothesis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24(2), 225-235.

Almy, B. and Krueger, J.I. (2013). Game interrupted: the rationality of considering the future. Judgment and Decision Making, 8, 521-526.

Aloisi, A.M. and Bonifazi, M. (2006). Sex hormones, central nervous system and pain. Hormones and Behavior, 50(1), 1-7.

Altmann, E.M. (2001). Near-term memory in programming: a simulation-based analysis. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 54, 189-210.

Altmann, E.M. and Trafton, J.G. (1999). Memory for goals: an architectural perspective. Proceedings of the Twenty First Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Alzheimer’s Society (n.d.). What is Korsakoff’s Syndrome? Retrieved 6 December 2015 from www.alzheimers.org.uk/site/scripts/documents_info.php?documentID=98

Ambler, C. and Milne, R. (2006). Call handling centres - an evidential opportunity or threat? Paper presented at the Second International Investigative Interviewing Conference, Portsmouth (July 2006).

Ameri, A. (1999). The effects of cannabinoids on the brain. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behaviour, 64(2), 257-260.

Amir, N., Beard, C., Burns, M. and Bomyea, J. (2009). Attention modification program in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 28-33.

Amir, N., Bomyes, J. and Beard, C. (2010). The effect of single-session interpretation modification on attention bias in socially anxious individuals. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 178-182.

Anderson, M.C., Bjork, R.A. and Bjork, E.L. (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1063-1087.

Anderson, M.C., Bjork, R.A. and Bjork, E.L. (2000). Retrieval-induced forgetting: evidence for a recall-specific mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 7, 522-530.

Anderson, M.C. and Neely, J.H. (1996). Interference and inhibition in memory retrieval. In E.L. Bjork and R.A. Bjork (eds), Memory: Handbook of perception and cognition (pp. 237-313). New York: Academic Press.

Arendt, J. (2009). Managing jet lag: some of the problems and possible new solutions. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 13(4), 249-256.

Arendt, J. (2010). Shift work: coping with the biological clock. Occupational Medicine, 60(1), 10-20.

Arnold, M.E., Petros, T.V., Beckwith, B.E., Coons, G. and Gorman, N. (1987). The effects of caffeine, impulsivity and sex on memory for word lists. Physiology and Behaviour, 41, 25-30.

Ashcraft, M.H. (1995). Cognitive psychology and simple arithmetic: a review and summary of new directions. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 3-34.

Ashcraft, M.H. and Kirk, E.P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental-Psychology: General, 130, 224-237.

Asso, D. (1983). The real menstrual cycle. London: Wiley.

Asso, D. (1987). Cyclical variations. In M.A. Baker (ed) Sex differences in human performance. New York: Wiley.

Atkinson, R.C. (1975). Mnemotechnics in second language learning. American Psychologist, 30, 821-828.

Atwood, M.E. and Polson, P.G. (1976). A process model for water jug problems. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 191-216.

Aubeeluck, A. and Maguire, M. (2002). The Menstrual Joy Questionnaire items alone can positively prime reporting of menstrual attitudes and symptoms. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26(2), 160-162.

Bachmann, L.M., Kolb, E., Koller, M.T., Steurer, J. and ter Riet, G. (2003). Accuracy of Ottawa ankle rules to exclude fractures of the ankle and mid-foot: systematic review. British Medical Journal, 326, 417.

Baddeley, A.D. (1982). Your memory: A user’s guide. New York: MacMillan.

Baddeley, A.D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Baddeley, A.D. (1993). Human memory: Theory and practice. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Baddeley, A.D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 5-28.

Baddeley, A.D. (1997). Human memory: Theory and practice (revised edition). Hove: Psychology Press.

Baddeley, A.D. (2001). Is working memory still working? American Psychologist, 56, 851-864.

Baddeley, A.D. (2012). Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1-29.

Baddeley, A.D., Allen, R.J. and Hitch, G. (2010). Investigating the episodic buffer. Psychologica Belgica, 50, 223-243.

Baddeley, A.D. and Hitch, G.J. (1974). Working memory. In G.H. Bower (ed), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (vol. 8). New York: Academic Press.

Baddeley, A.D. and Logie, R.H. (1999). Working memory: the multiple-component model. In A. Miyake and P. Shah (eds), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Baddeley, A.D. and Longman, D.J.A. (1978). The influence of length and frequency of training sessions on the rate of learning to type. Ergonomics, 21, 627-635.

Baddeley, A.D. and Wilson, B. (1988). Comprehension and working memory: a single case neuropsychological study. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 479-498.

Baehr, E.K., Revelle, W. and Eastman, C.I. (2000). Individual differences in the phase and amplitude of the human circadian temperature rhythm: with an emphasis on morningness-eveningness. Journal of Sleep Research, 9(2), 117-127.

Bahrick, H.P., Bahrick, P.O. and Wittlinger, R.P. (1975). Fifty years of memory for names and faces: a cross-sectional approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 54-75.

Bahrick, H.P., Hall, L.K. and Da Costa, L.A. (2008). Fifty years of memory for college grades. Emotion, 8, 13-22.

Bailes, F.A. (2006). The use of experience-sampling methods to monitor musical imagery in everyday life. Musicae Scientiae, 10(2), 173-190.

Baird, A. and Samson, S. (2015). Music and dementia. Progress in Brain Research, 217, 207-235.

Baizer, J.S., Ungerleider, L.G. and Desimone, R. (1991). Organization of visual inputs to the inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in macaques. Journal of Neuroscience, 11(1), 168-190.

Baker, J. and Young, B. (2014). 20 years later: deliberate practice and the development of expertise in sport. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 7, 135-157, doi: 10.1080/1750984X.2014.896024

Baker, K.L., Williams, S. and Nicholson, R.I. (2000). Evaluating frequency proximity in stream segregation. Perception and Psychophysics, 62, 81-88.

Baker, M.A. (1987). Sensory functioning. In M.A. Baker (ed), Sex differences in human performance. New York: Wiley.

Balkin, T.J., Braun, A.R., Wesensten, N.J., Jeffries, K., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., Belenky, G. and Herscovitch, P. (2002). The process of awakening: a PET study of regional brain activity patterns mediating the re-establishment of alertness and consciousness. Brain, 125(10), 2308-2319.

Ballas, J.A. and Howard, J.H. (1987). Interpreting the language of environmental sounds. Environment and Behaviour, 19, 91-114.

Banaji, M.R. and Crowder, R.C. (1989). The bankruptcy of everyday memory. American Psychologist, 44, 1185-1193.

Banks, S. and Dinges, D.F. (2007). Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 3(5), 519-528.

Barbey, A.K. and Sloman, S.A. (2007). Base rate respect: from ecological rationality to dual processes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 241-297.

Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kronenburg, M.J. and van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1-24.

Barras, S. and Zehner, B. (2000). Responsive sonification of well logs. Proceedings of the International Community for Auditory Display. www.icad.org/home. Available from www.icad.org/websiteV2.0/Conferences/ICAD2000/ICAD2000.html, 12 May 2015.

Barron, G. and Erev, I. (2003). Small feedback-based decisions and their limited correspondence to description-based decisions. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 16, 215-233.

Barry, R.J, Clarke, A.R., Johnstone, S.J. and Rushby, J.A. (2008). Timing of caffeine’s impact on autonomic and central nervous system measures: clarification of arousal effects, Biological Psychology, 77(3), 304-316.

Bartlett, F.C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Basner, M., Rao, H., Goel, N. and Dinges, D.F. (2013). Sleep deprivation and neurobehavioral dynamics. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(5), 854-863.

Bauer, P.J. and Larkina, M. (2013). The onset of childhood amnesia in childhood: a prospective investigation of the course and determinants of forgetting of early-life events. Memory, 22(8), 907-924.

Baughman, B. (2015). A study of rape investigation files. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Huddersfield.

Baumeister, R.F. (1984). Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skilful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 610-620.

Baumeister, R.F. and Showers, C.J. (1986). A review of paradoxical performance effects - choking under pressure in sports and mental tests. European Journal of Social Psychology, 16, 361-383.

Bäuml, K-H. and Dobler, I.M. (2015). The two faces of selective memory retrieval: recall specificity of the detrimental but not the beneficial effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 1, 246-253.

Baur, B., Uttner, I., Ilmberger, J., Fesl, G. and Mai, N. (2000). Music memory provides access to verbal knowledge in a patient with global amnesia. Neurocase, 2000(6), 415-421.

Bedi, G. and Redman, J. (2008). Ecstasy use and higher-level cognitive functions: weak effects of ecstasy after control for potential confounds. Psychological Medicine, 38(9), 1319-1330.

Behan, M. and Wilson, M. (2008). State anxiety and visual attention: the role of the quiet eye period in aiming to a far target. Journal of Sports Sciences, 26, 207-215.

Beilock, S.L., Bertenthal, B.I., McCoy, A.M. and Carr, T.J. (2004). Haste does not always make waste: expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 373-379.

Beilock, S.L. and Carr, T.H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 701-725.

Beilock, S.L. and Gray, R. (2012). From attentional control to attentional spillover: a skill-level investigation of attention, movement, and performance outcomes. Human Movement Science, 31, 1473-1499.

Bendixen, A., Schroger, E. and Winkler, I. (2009). I heard that coming: event-related potential evidence for stimulus-driven prediction in the auditory system. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 8447-8451.

Benton, T.R., Ross, D.F., Bradshaw, E., Thomas, W.N. and Bradshaw, G.S. (2006). Eyewitness memory is still not common sense: comparing jurors, judges and law enforcement to eyewitness experts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 115-129.

Berenbaum, H. (2010). An initiation-termination two-phase model of worrying. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 962-975.

Berenbaum, H., Thompson, R.J. and Bredemeier, K. (2007). Perceived threat: exploring its association with worry and its hypothesised antecedents. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2473-2482.

Bergersen, G.R. and Gustafsson, J-E. (2011). Programming skill, knowledge and working memory among professional software developers from an investment theory perspective. Journal of Individual Differences, 32, 201-209.

Berridge, V., Herring, R. and Thom, B. (2009). Binge Drinking: A Confused Concept and its Contemporary History. Social Hisory of Medicine, 22(3), 597-607.

Bhatara, A., Tirovolas, A.K., Duan, L.M., Levy, B. and Levitin, D.J. (2011). Perception of emotional expression in musical performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(3), 921-934.

Bialystok, E., Poarch, G. Luo, L. and Craik, F.I.M. (2014). Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and Aging, 29, 696-705.

Bifulco, M. and Pisanti, S. (2015). Medicinal use of cannabis in Europe, EMBO reports, 16(2), 130-132.

Bindemann, M., Brown, C., Koyas, T. and Russ, A. (2012). Individual differences in face identification postdict eyewitness accuracy. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1(2), 96-103.

Bjork, R.A. (1999). Assessing our own competence: heuristics and illusions. In D. Gopher and A. Koriat (eds), Attention and performance XVII. Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application (pp. 435-459), Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Bjork, R.A. and Bjork, E.L. (1992). A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In A. Healey, S. Kosslyn and R. Shiffrin (eds), From learning processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honour of William K. Estes (vol. 2, pp. 35-67). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Black, S.L. and Koulis-Chitwood, A. (1990). The menstrual cycle and typing skill: an ecologically valid study of the ‘raging hormones’ hypothesis. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 22(4), 445-455.

Blask, D.E. (2009). Melatonin, sleep disturbance and cancer risk. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 13(4), 257-264.

Blum, K., Chen, A.L., Giordano, J., Borsten, J., Chen, T.J., Hauser, M., Simpatico, T., Femino, J., Braverman, E.R. and Barh, D. (2012). The addictive brain: all roads lead to dopamine. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 44(2), 134-143.

Boer, D., Fischer, R., Tekman, H.G., Abubakar, A., Njenga, J. and Markus Zenger, M. (2012). Young people’s topography of musical functions: personal, social and cultural experiences with music across genders and six societies. Inter national Journal of Psychology, 47(5), 335-369.

Boileau, I., Assaad, J.M., Pihl, R.O., Benkelfat, C., Leyton, M., Diksic, M., Tremblay, R.E. and Dagher, A. (2003). Alcohol promotes dopamine release in the human nucleus accumbens. Synapse, 49(4), 226-231.

Boivin, D.B. and James, F.O. (2002). Circadian adaptation to night-shift work by judicious light and darkness exposure. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 17(6), 556-567.

Bonnet, M.H. and Arnaud, D.L. (1994). The use of prophylactic naps and caffeine to maintain performance during a continuous operation. Ergonomics, 37(6), 1009-1020.

Bornstein, B.H., Deffenbacher, K.A., Penrod, S.D. and McGorty, E.K. (2012). Effects of exposure time and cognitive operations on facial identification accuracy: a meta-analysis of two variables associated with initial memory strength. Psychology, Crime & Law, 18(5), 473-490.

Bourne, C., Mavkay, C.E. and Holmes, E.A. (2013). The neural basis of flashback information: the impact of viewing trauma. Psychological Medicine, 43, 1521-1532.

Bower, G.H. (1970). Imagery as a relational organiser in associative learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 9, 529-533.

Bower, G.H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129-148.

Brace, N., Pike, G. and Kemp, R. (2000). Investigating E-FIT using famous faces. In A. Czerederecka, T. Jaskiewicz-Obydzinska and J. Wojcikiewicz (eds), Forensic Psychology and Law (pp. 272-276). Krakow: Krakow Institute of Forensic Research Publishers.

Brace, N., Pike, G., Kemp, R., Turner, J. and Bennett, P. (2006). Does the presentation of multiple facial composites improve suspect identification? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 213-226.

Braver, T.S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual mechanisms’ framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 106-113.

Braver, T.S. and West, R. (2008). Working memory, executive control and aging. In F.I.M. Craik and T.A. Salthouse (eds), The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd edn). New York: LEA.

Brazil, E. and Fernström, M. (2011). Auditory icons. In T. Hermann, A. Hunt and J.G. Neuhoff (eds), The sonification handbook. Berlin: Logos.

Bregman, A.S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Bregman, A.S. (1993). Auditory scene analysis: hearing in complex environments. In S. MacAdams and E. Bigand (eds), Thinking in sound: The cognitive psychology of human audition. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Bregman, A.S. and Pinker, S. (1978). Auditory streaming and the building of timbre. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 32, 19-31.

Brehmer, Y., Westerberg, H. and Backman, L. (2012). Working-memory training in younger and older adults: training gains, transfer, and maintenance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 63. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00063.

Brewer, N. and Wells, G.L. (2011). Eyewitness identification. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 24-27.

Brewster, S.A, Wright, P.C. and Edwards, A.D.N. (1992). A detailed investigation into the effectiveness of earcons. In G. Kramer (ed), Auditory display, sonification, audification and auditory interfaces: Proceedings of the First International Conference on Auditory Display. Santa Fe, NM: Addison-Wesley.

Bridgeman, B., Peery, S. and Anand, S. (1997). Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space. Perception and Psychophysics, 59(3), 456-469.

Brigham, J.C. and Wolfskeil, M.P. (1983). Opinions of attorneys and law enforcement personnel on the accuracy of eyewitness identifications. Law and Human Behavior, 7(4), 337-349.

Broadbent, D.E. (1958). Perception and communication. Oxford: Pergamon.

Broadbent, D.E. (1980). The minimisation of models. In A.J. Chapman and D.M. Jones (eds), Models of man. Leicester: British Psychological Society.

Broadway, J.M. and Engle, R.W. (2010). Validating running memory span: measurement of working memory capacity and links with fluid intelligence. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 563-570.

Broman, M. (2001). Spaced retrieval: a behavioural approach to memory improvement in Alzheimer’s and related dementias. NYS-Psychologist, 13, 31-34.

Brooks, A.W. (2014). Get excited: reappraising pre-performance anxiety as excitement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 1144-1158.

Brown, C., Lloyd-Jones, T.J. and Robinson, M. (2008). Eliciting person descriptions from eyewitnesses: a survey of police perceptions of eyewitness performance and reported use of interview techniques. The European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20, 529-560.

Brown, I.D. (2005). Review of the ‘Looked but Failed to See’ accident causation factor (I.B. Associates, trans.). London: Department for Transport.

Brown, I.D., Tickner, A.H. and Simmonds, D.C.V. (1969). Interference between concurrent tasks of driving and telephoning. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53(5), 419-424.

Brown, R. and Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5, 73-99.

Bruce, V. (1982). Changing faces: visual and non-visual coding processes in face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 73(1), 105-116.

Bruce, V., Henderson, Z., Greenwood, K., Hancock, P.J., Burton, A.M. and Miller, P. (1999). Verification of face identities from images captured on video. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 5(4), 339-360.

Bruce, V. and Young, A. (1986). Understanding face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 77(3), 305-327.

Brumback, T., Cao, D. and King, A. (2007). Effects of alcohol on psychomotor performance and perceived impairment in heavy binge social drinkers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 91(1), 10-17.

Budson, A.E. and Gold, C.A. (2009). Flashbulb, personal, and event memories in clinical populations. In O. Luminet and A. Curci (eds), Flashbulb memories: New issues and perspectives. Hove: Psychology Press.

Bugos, J.A., Perlstein, W.M., McCrae, C.S., Brophy, T.S. and Bedenbaugh, P.H. (2007). Individualized piano instruction enhances executive functioning and working memory in older adults. Aging and Mental Health, 11, 464-471.

Buijs, R.M., Van Eden, C.G., Gonchraruk, V.D. and Kalsbeek, A. (2003). The biological clock tunes the organs of the body: timing by hormones and the autonomic nervous system. Journal of Endocrinology, 177, 17-26.

Bull, R. (2004). Training: general principles and problems regarding behavioural cues to deception. In P.A. Granhag and L. Stromwall (eds), Detecting deception in forensic contexts (pp. 251-268). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Bull, R. (2010). The investigative interviewing of children and other vulnerable witnesses: psychological research and working/professional practice. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 15, 5-23.

Bull, R. (2013). What is ‘believed’ or actually ‘known’ about characteristics that may contribute to being a good/effective interviewer? Investigative Interviewing: Research and Practice, 5, 128-143.

Bull, R. (2014). When in interviews to disclose information to suspects and to challenge them? In R. Bull (ed), Investigative interviewing (pp. 167-181). New York: Springer.

Bull, R. and Clifford, R. (1984). Earwitness voice recognition accuracy. In G.L. Wells and E.F. Loftus (eds), Eyewitness testimony: Psychological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Bull, R. and Soukara, S. (2010). A set of studies of what really happens in police interviews with suspects. In G.D. Lassiter and C. Meissner (eds), Interrogations and confessions (pp. 81-95). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Bullier, J. and Nowak, L.G. (1995). Parallel versus serial processing: new vistas on the distributed organization of the visual system. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5(4), 497-503.

Burch, J.B., Yost, M.G., Johnson, W. and Allen, E. (2005). Melatonin, sleep, and shift work adaptation. Journal of Occupational and Environ mental Medicine, 47(9), 893-901.

Burian, S.E., Ligouri, A. and Robinson, J.H. (2002). Effects of alcohol on risk taking during simulated driving. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 17(3), 141-150.

Burke, D. and Yeadon, F. (2009). A comparison of compensation for release timing and maximum hand speed in recreational and competitive darts players. 27th International Conference on Biomechanics in Sports. Limerick, Ireland.

Burke, T.M., Scheer, F.A., Ronda, J.M., Czeisler, C.A. and Wright Jr, K.P. (2015). Sleep inertia, sleep homeostatic and circadian influences on higher-order cognitive functions. Journal of Sleep Research, doi: 10.1111/jsr.12291

Burton, A.M., Jenkins, R., Hancock, P.J. and White, D. (2005). Robust representations for face recognition: the power of averages. Cognitive Psychology, 51(3), 256-284.

Burton, A.M., Schweinberger, S., Jenkins, R. and Kaufmann, J. (2015). Arguments against a ‘configural processing’ account of familiar face recognition. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10 (4), 482-496.

Burton, A.M., White, D. and McNeill, A. (2010). The Glasgow face matching test. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 286-291.

Burton, A.M., Wilson, S., Cowan, M. and Bruce, V. (1999). Face recognition in poor-quality video: evidence from security surveillance. Psychological Science, 10(3), 243-248.

Burton, R., Thomson, F., Visintin, C. and Wright, C. (2014). United Kingdom drug situation: annual report to the European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction (EMCDDA) 2014. UK Focal point on drugs. Available from www.tiaft.org/uk-focal-point-report-2014.html [accessed 5 May 2015].

Bussemakers, M.P. and de Haan, A. (2000). When it sounds like a duck and it looks like a duck … Auditory icons vs earcons in multimedia environments. In P.R. Cook (ed), Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Auditory Display. Atlanta, GA: International Community for Auditory Display.

Buzan, T. (1974). Use your head. London: BBC books.

Byrne, M.D. and Bovair, S. (1997). A working memory model of a common procedural error. Cognitive Science, 21, 31-62.

Cady, E.T., Harris, R.J. and Knappenberger, J.B. (2008). Using music to cue autobiographical memories of different lifetime periods. Psychology of Music, 36(2), 157-177.

Cairney, P. and Catchpole, J. (1996). Patterns of perceptual failures at intersections of arterial roads and local streets. In A.G. Gale, I.D. Brown, C.M. Haslegrave and S.P. Taylor (eds), Vision in Vehicles V. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

Calmeiro, L., Tenenbaum, G. and Eccles, D.W. (2014). Managing pressure: patterns of appraisals and coping strategies of non-elite and elite athletes during competition. Journal of Sports Sciences, 32, 1813-1820.

Camerer, C.F. (2000). Prospect theory in the wild: evidence from the field. In Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (eds), Choices, values, and frames. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Camilleri, A.R. and Larrick, R.P. (2015). Choice architecture. In R. Scott and S. Kosslen (eds), Emerging trends in the social and behavioral sciences. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Cañal-Bruland, R., Pijpers, J.R. and Oudejans, R.R.D. (2010). The influence of anxiety on action-specific perception. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 23, 353-361.

Candel, I., Jelicik, M., Merckelbach, H. and Wester, A. (2003). Korsakoff patients’ memories of September 11, 2001. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 191, 262-265.

Carpenter, S.K. and Pashler, H. (2007). Testing beyond words: using tests to enhance visuospatial map learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 474-478.

Carpenter, S.K., Pashler, H., Wixted, J.T. and Vul, E. (2008). The effect of tests on learning and forgetting. Memory & Cognition, 36, 438-448.

Cashmore, J. and Trimboli, L. (2005). An evaluation of the NSW child sexual assault special jurisdiction pilot. Sydney: NSW Bureau of Crime Statistics and Research.

Casscells, W., Schoenberger, A. and Grayboys, T. (1978). Interpretation by physicians of clinical laboratory results. New England Journal of Medicine, 299, 999-1001.

Castiello, U. and Umiltà, C. (1992). Orienting of attention in volleyball players. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 23, 301-310.

Cataldo, J., Prochaska, J. and Glantz, S.A. (2010). Cigarette smoking is a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis controlling for tobacco industry affiliation. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 19 (2), 465-480.

Cauli, O. and Morelli, M. (2005). Caffeine and the dopaminergic system. Behavioural Pharmacology, 16(2), 63-77.

Causer, J., Holmes, P.S., Smith, N.C. and Williams, A.M. (2011). Anxiety, movement kinematics, and visual attention in elite-level performers. Emotion, 11, 595-602.

Cepeda, N.J., Vul, E., Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. and Pashler, H. (2008). Spacing effects in learning. Psychological Science, 19, 1095-1102.

Chan, J.C.K. and LaPaglia, J.A. (2013). Impairing existing declarative memory in humans by disrupting reconsolidation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(23), 9309-9313.

Chang, H.W. and Trehub, S.E. (1977a) Auditory processing of relational information by young infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 24, 324-331.

Chang, H.W. and Trehub, S.E. (1977b) Infants’ perception of temporal grouping in auditory patterns. Child Development, 48, 1666-1670.

Chapman, G.B. and Johnson, E.J. (2002). Incorporating the irrelevant: anchors in judgments of belief and value. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin and D. Kahneman (eds), The psychology of intuitive judgment: Heuristics and biases (pp. 120-138). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Charlton, S.G. (2009). Driving while conversing: cell phones that distract and passengers who react. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 41(1), 160.

Chase, W.G. and Ericsson, K.A. (1981). Skilled memory. In J.R. Anderson (ed), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 141-189). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Chase, W.G. and Ericsson, K.A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In G.H. Bower (ed), The psychology of learning and motivation (vol. 16. pp. 1-58). New York: Academic Press.

Cheng, W.N.K., Hardy, L. and Markland, D. (2009). Toward a three-dimensional conceptualisation of performance anxiety: rationale and initial measurement development. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 271-278.

Chentouf, Z. (2014). Cognitive software engineering: a research framework and roadmap. Journal of Software Engineering and Applications, 7, 530-539.

Cherry, E.C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech with one and two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975-979.

Chincotta, D., Underwood, G., Ghani, K.A., Papadopoulou, E. and Wresinki, M. (1999). Memory span for Arabic numerals and digit words: evidence for a limited capacity visuo-spatial storage system. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 325-351.

Cho, K. (2001). Chronic ‘jet lag’ produces temporal lobe atrophy and spatial cognitive deficits. Nature Neuroscience, 4(6), 567-568.

Cho, K., Ennaceur, A., Cole, J.C. and Suh, C.K. (2000). Chronic jet lag produces cognitive deficits. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(6), 1-5.

Chrisler, J.C., Johnston, I.K., Champagne, N.M. and Preston, K.E. (1994). Menstrual joy: the construct and its consequences. Psychology of Women Quarterly 18, 375-388.

Christie, D.F. and Ellis, H.D. (1981). Photofit constructions versus verbal descriptions of faces. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66(3), 358-363.

Chu, S. and Downes, J.J. (2000). Long live Proust: the odour-cued autobiographical memory bump. Cognition, 75, B41-B50.

Ciocca, V. and Bregman, A.S. (1987). Perceived continuity of gliding and steady-state tones through interrupting noise. Perception and Psychophysics, 42, 476-484.

Cipriani, G., Bianchette, A. and Trabucchi, M. (2006). Outcomes of a computer-based cognitive rehabilitation program on Alzheimer’s disease patients compared with those of patients affected by mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 43, 327-335.

Clark, H.H. and Clarke, E.V. (1977). Psychology and language. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Clarke, C. and Milne, R. (2001). National evaluation of the PEACE investigative interviewing course, Police Research Award Scheme, PRAS/149. London: Home Office.

Clifford, B. and George, R. (1996). A field investigation of training in three methods of witness/victim investigative interviewing. Psychology, Crime & Law, 2, 231-248.

Clifford, B.R. and Bull, R. (1978). The psychology of person identification. London: Routledge.

Clow, A., Law, R., Evans, P., Vallence, A.M., Hodyl, N.A., Goldsworthy, M.R., Rothwell, J.R. and Ridding, M.C. (2014). Day differences in the cortisol awakening response predict day differences in synaptic plasticity in the brain. Stress, 17(3), 219-223.

Colcombe, S.J., Erickson, K.I., Scalf, P.E., Kim, J.P., Prakash, R., McAuley, E., Elavsky, S., Marquez, D.X., Hu, L. and Kramer, A.F. (2006). Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 61A, (11), 1166-1700.

Cole, B.L. and Hughes, P.K. (1984). A field trial of attention and search conspicuity. Human Factors, 26(3), 299-313.

Cole, J., Sumnall, H.R. and Grob, C.S. (2002). Sorted: ecstasy facts and fiction. The Psychologist, 15(9), 464-467.

Cole, L.A., Ladner, D.G. and Byrn, F.W. (2009). The normal variabilities of the menstrual cycle. Fertility and Sterility, 91(2), 522-527. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.fertnstert.2007.11.073 [accessed 4 December 2015].

Collins, P., Maguire, M. and O’Dell, L. (2002). Smokers’ representations of their own smoking: a Q-methodological study. Journal of Health Psychology, 7(6), 641-652.

Conway, M.A. (1991). Cognitive psychology in search of meaning: the study of autobiographical memory. The Psychologist, 4, 301-305.

Conway, M.A. (1995). Flashbulb memories. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Conway, M.A. (2005). Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 594-628.

Conway, M.A., Anderson, S.J., Larsen, S.F., Donelly, C.M., McDaniel, M.A., McCleland, A.G.R. and Rawls, R.E. (1994). The formation of flashbulb memories. Memory & Cognition, 22, 326-343.

Conway, M.A. and Jobson, L. (2012). On the nature of autobiographical memory. In D. Berntsen and D.C. Rubin (eds), Understanding autobiographical memory: Theories and approaches. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Conway, M.A., Wang, Q., Hanyu, K. and Haque, S. (2005). A cross-cultural investigation of autobiographical memory—on the universality and cultural variation of the reminiscence bump. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 36, 739-749.

Cook, S. and Wilding, J. (1997a). Earwitness testimony: never mind the variety, hear the length. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 95-111.

Cook, S. and Wilding, J. (1997b). Earwitness testimony 2: voices, faces and context. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 527-541.

Cooke, A., Kavussanu, M., McIntyre, D. and Ring, C. (2010). Psychological, muscular and kinematic factors mediate performance under pressure. Psychophysiology, 47, 1109-1118.

Corbetta, M. and Shulman, G.L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 201-215.

Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (1996). Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment under uncertainty. Cognition, 58, 1-73.

Cowan, N. (2005). Working memory capacity. Hove: Psychology Press.

Craik, F.I.M. (1977). Depth of processing in recall and recognition. In S. Dornik (ed), Attention and performance (vol. 6, pp. 679-698). New York: Raven Press.

Craik, F.I.M. (2002). Levels of Processing: past, present…and future? Memory, 10, 305-318.

Craik, F.I.M., Anderson, N.D., Kerr, S.A. and Li, K.Z.H. (1995). Memory changes in normal ageing. In A.D. Baddeley, B.A. Wilson and F.N. Watts (eds), Handbook of memory disorders. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.

Craik, F.I.M. and Lockhart, R.S. (1972). Levels of processing: a framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 11, 671-684.

Craik, F.I.M. and Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268-294.

Crean, R., Crane, N. and Mason, B. (2011). An evidence based review of acute and long-term effects of cannabis use on executive cognitive functions. Journal of Addiction Medicine, 5(1), 1-8.

Creem, S.H. and Proffitt, D.R. (2001). Defining the cortical visual systems: ‘what’, ‘where’, and ‘how’. Acta Psychologica, 107, 43-68.

Cristea, I.A., Kok, R.N. and Cuijpers, P. (2015). Efficacy of cognitive bias modification interventions in anxiety and depression: meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 206, 1, 7-16.

Croft, R.J., Mackay, A.J., Mills, A.T.D. and Gruzeiler, J.G.D. (2001). The relative contributions of ecstasy and cannabis to cognitive impairment. Psychopharmacology, 153, 373-379.

Crovitz, H.F. and Schiffman, H. (1974). Frequency of episodic memories as a function of their age. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4, 517-518.

Cuddy, L.L. and Duffin, J. (2005). Music, memory, and Alzheimer’s disease: is music recognition spared in dementia, and how can it be assessed? Medical Hypotheses, 64(2), 229-235.

Curran, V. (2000). Is MDMA (ecstasy) neurotoxic in humans? An overview of evidence and methodological problems in research. Neuropsychobiology, 42, 34-41.

Cutler, B.L., Penrod, S.D. and Dexter, H.R. (1989). The eyewitness, the expert psychologist, and the jury. Law and Human Behavior, 13(3), 311-332.

Cutler, B.L., Penrod, S.D. and Dexter, H.R. (1990). Juror sensitivity to eyewitness identification evidence. Law and Human Behavior, 14(2), 185-191.

Cutting, J.E. and Rosner, B.S. (1976). Discrimination functions predicted from categories in speech and music. Perception and Psychophysics, 20, 87-88.

Czeisler, C.A., Duffy, J.F., Shanahan, T.L., Brown, E.N., Mitchell, J.F., Rimmer, D.W., Ronda, J.M., Silva, E.J., Allan, J.S., Emens, J.S., Dijk, D.J. and Kronauer, R.E. (1999). Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker. Science, 284(5423), 2177-2181.

Dalla Bella, S., Peretz, I., Rousseau, L. and Gosselin, N. (2001). A developmental study of the affective value of tempo and mode in music. Cognition, 80(3), B1-B10.

Dalton, G. and Ang, J. (2014). An evaluation of how police forces utilise body worn video cameras. Singapore: Home Team Behavioural Sciences Centre, Research Report: Singapore Police.

Dalton, G., Milne, R., Hope, L. and Pike, G. (in prep.). Body worn video cameras: an evaluation of police frontline communication.

Dalton, K. (1960). Effect of menstruation on schoolgirls’ weekly work. British Medical Journal, i, 326-328.

Dalton, K. (1968). Menstruation and examinations. The Lancet, 11, 1386-1388.

Dando, C. (2013). Drawing to remember: external support of older adults’ eyewitness performance. PLOS ONE, 8, e69937.

Dando, C. and Bull, R. (2011). Maximising opportunities to detect verbal deception: training police officers to interview tactically. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 8, 189-202.

Dando, C., Bull, R., Ormerod, T. and Sandham, A. (2015). Helping to sort the liars from the truth-tellers: the gradual revelation of information during investigative interviews. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 20, 114-128.

Dando, C., Geiselman, R.E., Macleod, N. and Griffiths, A. (in press). Interviewing adult witnesses and victims. In G. Oxburgh, T. Myklebust, T. Grant and R. Milne (eds), Communication in legal contexts: A handbook. Chichester: Wiley.

Dando, C.J., Wilcock, R. and Milne, R. (2009b). The Cognitive Interview: the efficacy of a modified mental reinstatement of context procedure for frontline police investigators. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 138-147.

Dando, C.J., Wilcock, R. and Milne, R. (2008). The cognitive interview: inexperienced police officers’ perceptions of their witness/victim interviewing practices. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 13, 59-70.

Dando, C.J., Wilcock, R. and Milne, R. (2009a). The cognitive interview: novice police officers’ witness/victim interviewing practices. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 15, 679-696.

Dando, C.J., Wilcock, R., Milne, R. and Henry, L. (2009c). A modified cognitive interview procedure for frontline police investigators. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 698-716.

Daneman, M. and Carpenter, P.A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 19, 450-466.

Dark, V.J. and Benbow, C.P. (1991). Differential enhancement of working memory with mathemat ical versus verbal precocity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 48-60.

Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man. London: John Murray.

Das, M. and Chattopadhyay, P.K. (1982). Arousal in menstruation: a study with some CNS measures. Indian Journal of Clinical Psychology, 9(2), 99-104.

Davidson, P.S.R. and Glisky, E.L. (2002). Is flashbulb memory a special instance of source memory? Evidence form older adults. Memory, 10, 99-111.

Davies, G., Bull, R. and Milne, R. (2016). Analysing and improving the testimony of vul nerable witnesses interviewed under the ‘Achieving Best Evidence’ protocol. In P. Radcliffe, A. Heaton-Armstrong, G. Gudjonsson and D. Wolchover (eds), Witness testimony in sexual cases: Investigation, law and procedure. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Davies, G.M. and Patel, D. (2005). The influence of car and driver stereotypes on attributions of vehicle speed, position on the road and culpability in a road accident scenario. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 10, 45-62.

Davis, D. and Loftus, E.L. (2005). Age and functioning in the legal system: perception memory and judgement in victims, witnesses and jurors. In I. Noy and W. Karwowski (eds), Handbook of forensic human factors and ergonomics. London: Taylor & Francis.

Davis, J.P., Jansari, A. and Lander, K. (2013). ‘I never forget a face!’ The Psychologist, 26(10), 726-729.

Davis, J.P. and Valentine, T. (2009). CCTV on trial: matching video images with the defendant in the dock. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23(4), 482-505.

Dawes, R.M., Faust, D. and Meehl, P.E. (1989). Clinical versus actuarial judgment. Science, 243, 1668-1674.

De Bene, R. and Moe, A. (2003). Presentation modality effects in studying passages: are mental images always effective? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 309-324.

De Sa, V. (1999). Combining unimodal classifiers to improve learning. In H. Ritter, H. Cruse and J. Dean (eds) Pre-rational intelligence: Adaptive behavior and intelligent systems without symbols and logic (vol. 2). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

Deffenbacher, K.A., Bornstein, B.H., Penrod, S.D. and McGorty, E.K. (2004). A meta-analytic review of the effects of high stress on eyewitness memory. Law and Human Behavior, 28(6), 687-706.

deGroot, A.D. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague: Mouton Publishers.

Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44, 1-42.

Dehaene, S., Bossini, S. and Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371-396.

Dehaene, S. and Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatom ical and functional model of number processing. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 83-120.

Delaney, J., Lupton, M.J. and Toth, E. (1987). The curse: A cultural history of menstruation. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

Dement, W. and Kleitman, N. (1957). Cyclic variations in EEG during sleep and their relation to eye movements, body motility, and dreaming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neuropsychology, 9(4), 673-690.

Demiray, B. and Freund, A. (2015). Michael Jackson, Bin Laden and I: functions of positive and negative, public and private flashbulb memories. Memory, 23(4), 487-506.

Dempster, F.N. (1987). Effects of variable encoding and spaced presentations on vocabulary learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 162-170.

Department for Transport (DfT). (2014). Reported road casualties in Great Britain: Main results 2013.

Department of Health (2012). Steering Group Report on a National Substance Misuse Strategy. Dublin: The Stationery Office. Available from www.drugsandalcohol.ie/16908/Steering_Group_Report-on-a_national_substance_misuse_strategy_7_Feb_11.pdf [accessed 5 May 2015.]

Derakshan, N., Ducrocqu, D., Wilson, M., Eysenck, M.W. and Berggren, N. (submitted). Training attentional control to improve tennis performance: transfer effects on field performance, distractibility, and inhibitory control.

Derntl, B., Schöpf, V., Kollndorfer, K. and Lanzenberger, R. (2012). Menstrual cycle phase and duration of oral contraception intake affect olfactory perception. Chemical Senses, 38(1), 67-75.

Derryberry, D. and Reed, M.A. (1998). Anxiety and attentional focusing: trait, state and hemispheric influences. Personality and Individual Differences, 25(4), 745-761. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869 (98)00117-2.

Derryberry, D. and Tucker, D.M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In P.M. Neidenthal and S. Kitayama (eds), The heart’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp. 167-196). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Deutsch, D. (1972). Octave generalization and tune recognition. Perception & Psychophysics, 11(6), 411-412.

Deutsch, D. (1975). Two-channel listening to musical scales. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 57, 1156-1160.

Devan, B.D., Goad, E.H., Petri, H.L., Antoniadis, E.A., Hong, N.S., Ko, C.H., Leblanc, L., Lebovic, S.S., Lo, Q. and Ralph, M.R. (2001). Circadian phase-shifted rats show normal acquisition but impaired long-term retention of place information in the water task. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 75, 51-62.

Devlin, Lord P. (1976). Report to the Secretary of State for the Home Department on the Departmental Committee on Evidence of Identification in Criminal Cases. London: HMSO.

Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M.W., Nordgren, L.F. and van Baaren, R.B. (2006). On making the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science, 311, 1005-1007.

Dinges, D.F. (1992). Adult napping and its effects on ability to function. In: C. Stampi (ed), Why we nap (pp. 118-134). Birkhäuser Boston.

Dingler, T., Lindsay, J. and Walker, B.N. (2008). Learnability of sound cues for environmental features: auditory icons, earcons, spearcons and speech. Proceedings of the 14th International Conference on Auditory Display, Paris, France (June 24-27).

Dix, A., Finlay, J., Abowd, G. and Beale, R. (1997). Human-computer interaction (2nd edn). Harlow: Prentice Hall.

Doane, L.D., Kremen, W.S., Eaves, L.J., Eisen, S.A., Hauger, R., Hellhammer, D., Levine, S., Lupien, S., Lyons, M.J., Mendoza, S., Prom-Wormley, E., Xian, H., York, T.P., Franz, C.E. and Jacobson, K.C. (2010). Associations between jet lag and cortisol diurnal rhythms after domestic travel. Health Psychology, 29, 117-123.

Dodson, C. and Reisberg, D. (1991). Indirect testing of eyewitness memory: the (non) effect of misinformation. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29, 333-336.

Dodson, C.S. and Krueger, L.E. (2006). I misremember it well: why older adults are unreliable witnesses. Psychonomic Bulletin and Revue, 13, 770-775.

Dombois, F. (2001). Using audification in planetary seismology. Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Auditory Display (ICAD2001), Espoo, Finland, July 29-August 1, 2001, eds: J. Hiipakka, N. Zacharov and T. Takala. International Community for Auditory Display.

Dougherty, M.R., Franco-Watkins, A.M. and Thomas, R. (2008). Psychological plausibility of the theory of probabilistic mental models and the fast and frugal heuristics. Psychological Review, 115, 199-211.

Dowling, W.J. (1973). The perception of interleaved melodies. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 322-337.

Drachman, D. (2005). Do we have brain to spare? Neurology, 64(12), 2004-2005.

Druckman, D. and Bjork, R.A. (1994). Learning, remembering, believing: Enhancing human performance. Washington, DC: National Acad emy Press.

Duffy, J.F., Dijk, D.J., Hall, E.F. and Czeisler, C.A. (1999). Relationship of endogenous circadian melatonin and temperature rhythms to self-reported preference for morning or evening activity in young and older people. Journal of Investigative Medicine: The Official Publication of the American Federation for Clinical Research, 47(3), 141-150.

Dumas, J.A. and Newhouse, P.A. (2012). The cholinergic hypothesis of cognitive aging revisited again: cholinergic functional compensation. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 99(2), 254-261.

Dumontheil, I. and Klingberg, T. (2012). Brain activity during a visuospatial working memory task predicts arithmetical performance 2 years later. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 1078-1108.

Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K.A., Marsh, E.J., Nathan, M.J. and Willingham, D.T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14, 4-58.

Durie, B. (2005). Doors of perception. New Scientist, 185(2484), 33-36.

Durmer, J.S. and Dinges, D.F. (2005). Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Seminars in Neurology, 25(1), 117-129.

Dyrenfurth, I., Jewelewicz, R., Warren, M., Ferin, M. and VandeWiele, R.L. (1974). Temporal relationships of hormonal variables in the menstrual cycle. In M.M. Ferin, F. Halberg, M. Richart, and R.L. VandeWiele (eds), Biorhythms and human reproduction. New York: Wiley.

Easterbrook, J.A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilisation and the organisation of behaviour. Psychological Review, 66, 183-201.

Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Uber das Gedachtnis: Untersuchugen zur Experimentellen Psychologie. Leipzig: Dunker & Humbolt.

Ebbinghaus, H. (1913). Memory; A contribution to experimental psychology. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University (Original work published in 1885).

Edelson, M., Sharot, T., Dolan, R.J. and Dudai, Y. (2011). Following the crowd: brain substrates of long-term memory conformity. Science, 333, 108-111.

Edmond, G., Martire, K., Kemp, R., Hamer, D., Hibbert, B., et al. (2014). How to cross-examine forensic scientists: a guide for lawyers. Australian Bar Review, 39, 174-197.

Edwards, B., Waterhouse, J. and Reilly, T. (2007). The effects of circadian rhythmicity and time-awake on a simple motor task. Chronobiology International, 24(6), 1109-1124.

Edwards, W. and Fasolo, B. (2001). Decision technology. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 581-606.

Ehrenreich, H., Rinn, T., Kunert, H.J., Moeller, M.R., Poser, W., Schilling, L., Gigerenzer, G. and Hoehe, M.R. (1999). Specific attentional dysfunction in adults following early start of cannabis use. Psychopharmacology, 142(3), 295-301).

Eisner, A., Burke, S.N. and Toomey, M.D. (2004). Visual sensitivity across the menstrual cycle. Visual Neuroscience, 21(4), 513-531. http://doi.org/10.10.17/S0952523804214031 [accessed 4 December 2015].

Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 169-200.

El Haj, M., Postal, V. and Allain, P. (2012). Music enhances autobiographical memory in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Educational Gerontology, 38(1), 30-41.

Ellis, D. (1995). Hard problems in computational auditory scene analysis. Posted to the AUDITORY email list, August. Available at: http://web.media.mit.edu/~dpwe/writing/hard-probs.html [retrieved 5 July 2004].

Ellis, H.D., Davies, G.M. and Shepherd, J.W. (1978). A critical examination of the photofit system for recalling faces. Ergonomics, 21(4), 297-307.

Emmorey, K., McCullough, S. and Brentan, D. (2003). Categorical perception in American Sign Language. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18 (1), 21-45.

Endsley, M.R. (1997). Distribution of attention, situation awareness and workload in a passive air traffic control task: implications for operational errors and automation. Technical Report, Federal Aviation Administration, Office of Aviation Med icine. Washington DC. DOT/FAA/AM-97/13.

Englander-Golden, P., Whitmore, M.R. and Dienstbier, R.A. (1978). Menstrual cycle as a focus of study and self-reports of moods and behaviors. Motivation and Emotion, 2(1), 75-86.

Engle, R.W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19-23.

Engle, R.W., Kane, M.J. and Tuholski, S.W. (1999). Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence, and functions of the prefrontal cortex. In A. Miyake and P. Shah (eds), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Englert, C. and Oudejans, R.R.D. (2014). Is choking under pressure a consequence of skill-focus or increased distractibility? Results from a tennis serve task. Psychology, 5, 1035-1043.

Epting, L.K. and Overman, W.H. (1998). Sex sensitive tasks in men and women: a search for performance fluctuations across the menstrual cycle. Behavioural Neuroscience, 112(6), 1304-1317.

Er, N. (2003). A new flashbulb memory model applied to the Marmara earthquake. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 503-517.

Erev, I. and Roth, A.E. (2014). Maximization, learning and economic behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 10818-10825.

Ericsson, K.A., Delaney, P.F., Weaver, G. and Mahadevan, R. (2004). Uncovering the structure of a mnemonist’s superior ‘basic’ memory capacity. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 191-237.

Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.T. and Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of musical performance. Psychological Review, 100(3), 363-406.

Eschrich, S., Münte, T.F. and Altenmüller, E.O. (2008). Unforgettable film music: the role of emotion in episodic long-term memory for music. BMC Neuroscience, 9(1), 48.

Estes, W.K. (1972). An associative basis for coding and organisation in memory. In A.W. Melton and E. Martin (eds), Coding processes in human memory. Washington, DC: Winston.

Evans, J.St.B.T., Handley, S.H., Perham, N., Over, D.E. and Thompson, V.A. (2000). Frequency versus probability formats in statistical word problems. Cognition, 77, 197-213.

Eysenck, H. J. (1991). Dimensions of personality: criteria for a taxonomic paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 773-790.

Eysenck, H.J. and Eysenck, M.W. (1985). Personality and individual differences: A natural science approach. New York: Plenum Press.

Eysenck, M.W. (1982). Attention and arousal: Attention and arousal. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Eysenck, M.W. (1997). Anxiety and cognition: A unified theory. Hove: Psychology Press.

Eysenck, M.W. and Calvo, M.G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: the processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409-434.

Eysenck, M.W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R. and Calvo, M.G. (2007). Anxiety and Cognitive Performance: attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336-353.

Eysenck, M.W. and Eysenck, M.C. (1980). Effects of processing depth, distinctiveness, and word frequency on retention. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 263-274.

Eysenck, M.W. and Keane, M.T. (2015). Cognitive psychology: A student’s handbook (7th edn). Hove: Psychology Press.

Eysenck, M.W., MacLeod, C. and Mathews, A. (1987). Cognitive functioning and anxiety. Psychological Research, 49, 189-195.

Fahsing, I., Ask, K. and Granhag, P-A. (2004). The man behind the mask: accuracy and predictors of eyewitness offender descriptions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 722-729.

Fales, C.L., Barch, D.M., Burgess, A., Schaefer, D.S., Mennin, J.R., Gray, J.R., et al. (2008). Anxiety and cognitive efficiency: differential modulation of transient and sustained neural activity during a working memory task. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 239-253.

Farah, M.J. (1996). Is face recognition ‘special’? Evidence from neuropsychology. Behavioural Brain Research, 76(1), 181-189.

Farris, E. (1956). Human ovulation and fertility. New York: Pitman.

Fawcett, J.M., Russell, E.J., Peace, K.A. and Christie, J. (2013). Of guns and geese: a meta-analytic review of the ‘weapon focus’ literature. Psychology, Crime & Law, 19(1), 35-66.

Fenner, J., Heathcote, D. and Jerrams-Smith, J. (2000). The development of wayfinding competency: asymmetrical effects of visuo-spatial and verbal ability. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 20, 165-175.

Ferrara, M., De Gennaro, L. and Bertini, M. (2000). Time-course of sleep inertia upon awakening from nighttime sleep with different sleep homeostasis conditions. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 71(3), 225-229.

Ficca, G., Axelsson, J., Mollicone, D.J., Muto, V. and Vitiello, M.V. (2010). Naps, cognition and performance, Sleep Medicine Reviews, 14(4), 249-258.

Fiedler, K. and Juslin, P. (2006). Taking the interface between mind and environment seriously. In K. Fiedler and P. Juslin (eds), Information sampling and adaptive cognition (pp. 3-29). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Fischer, S., Hallschmid, M., Elsner, A.L. and Born, J. (2002). Sleep forms memory for finger skills. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(18), 11987-11991.

Fisher, H.G. and Freedman, S.J. (1968). The role of the pinna in auditory localisation. Journal of Auditory Research, 8, 15-26.

Fisher, R.P., Chin, D.M. and McCauley, M.R. (1990). Enhancing eyewitness recollection with the cognitive interview. National Police Research Unit Review, 6, 3-11.

Fisher, R.P. and Geiselman, R.E. (1992). Memory-enhancing techniques for investigative interviewing: The cognitive interview. Springfield, IL: Charles Thomas.

Fisher, R.P. and Geiselman, R.E. (2010).The cognitive interview method of conducting police interviews: eliciting extensive information and promoting therapeutic jurisprudence. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 33, 321-328.

Fisher, R.P., Geiselman, R.E. and Amador, M. (1989). Field test of the cognitive interview: enhancing the recollection of actual victims and witness of crime. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 722-727.

Fisher, R.P., Geiselman, R.E. and Raymond, D.S. (1987). Critical analysis of police interviewing techniques. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 15, 177-185.

Fisher, R.P., Milne, R. and Bull, R. (2011). Interviewing cooperative witnesses. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 16-19.

Fisher, R.P., Ross, S.J. and Cahill, B.S. (2010). Interviewing witnesses and victims. In P-A. Granhag (ed), Forensic psychology in context; Nordic and international approaches (pp. 56-74). Cullompton: Willan.

Flegal, K.E. and Anderson, M.C. (2008). Overthinking skilled motor performance: or why those who teach can’t do. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 927-932.

Flin, R., Boon, J., Knox, A. and Bull, R. (1992). The effect of a five-month delay on children’s and adults’ eyewitness memory. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 323-336.

Flin, R., Bull, R., Boon, J. and Knox, A. (1993). Child witnesses in Scottish criminal trials. International Review of Victimology, 2, 309-329.

Folkard, S. (2008). Do permanent night workers show circadian adjustment? A review based on the endogenous melatonin rhythm. Chronobiology International, 25(2-3), 215-224.

Folkard, S. and Tucker, P. (2003). Shift work, safety and productivity. Occupational Medicine, 53(2), 95-101.

Fornazzari, L., Castle, T., Nadkarni, S., Ambrose, M., Miranda, D., Apanasiewicz, N. and Phillips, F. (2006). Preservation of episodic musical memory in a pianist with Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 66(4), 610-611.

Foster, J.L., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T.L., Hicks, K.L., Redick, T.S. and Engle, R.W. (2015). Shortened complex span tasks can reliably measure working memory capacity. Memory & Cognition, 43, 226-236.

Foster, N.A. and Valentine, E.R. (2001). The effect of auditory stimulation on autobiographical recall in dementia. Experimental Aging Research, 27(3), 215-228.

Fowler, F.D. (1980). Air traffic control problems: a pilot’s view. Human Factors, 22, 645-653.

Frankenhauser, M., Myrsten, A.L. and Post, B. (1970). Psychophysiological reactions to cigarette smoking. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 11, 237-245.

French, P. (1994). An overview of forensic phonetics with particular reference to speaker identification. Forensic Linguistics, 1, 169-181.

Frenda, S.J., Nichols, R.M. and Loftus, E.F. (2011). Current issued and advances in misinformation research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 20-23.

Frenda, S.J., Patihis, L., Loftus, E.F., Lewis, H.C. and Fenn, K.M. (2014). Sleep deprivation and false memories. Psychological Science, 25(9), 1674-1681.

Freud, S. (1938). Psychopathology of everyday life. In A.A. Brill (ed), The writings of Sigmund Freud. New York: Modern Library.

Freudenthal, D. (2001). Age differences in the performance of information retrieval tasks. Behaviour and Information Technology, 20, 9-22.

Frieler, K., Fischinger, T., Schlemmer, K., Lothwesen, K., Jakubowski, K. and Müllensiefen, D. (2013). Absolute memory for pitch: a comparative replication of Levitin’s 1994 study in six European labs. Musicae Scientiae, 17(3), 334-349.

Frowd, C., Bruce, V., Ross, D., McIntyre, A. and Hancock, P.J. (2007). An application of caricature: how to improve the recognition of facial composites. Visual Cognition, 15(8), 954-984.

Frowd, C.D., Erickson, W.B., Lampinen, J.M., Skelton, F.C., McIntyre, A.H. and Hancock, P.J. (2015). A decade of evolving composite techniques: regression-and meta-analysis. Journal of Forensic Practice, 17(4), 319-334.

Frowd, C., Skelton, F., Hepton, G., Holden, L., Minahil, S., Pitchford, M. and Hancock, P.J. (2013). Whole-face procedures for recovering facial images from memory. Science & Justice, 53(2), 89-97.

Fruzzetti, A.E., Toland, K., Teller, S.A. and Loftus, E.F. (1992). Memory and eyewitness testimony. In M. Gruneberg and P. Morris (eds), Aspects of memory, Vol. 1: The practical aspects. New York: Routledge.

Frye, C.A. and Demolar, G.L. (1994). Menstrual cycle and sex differences influence salt preference. Physiology & Behavior, 55(1), 193-197.

Fuerst, A.J. and Hitch, G.J. (2000). Separate roles for executive and phonological components of working memory in mental arithmetic. Memory & Cognition, 28, 774-782.

Gabbert, F. and Hope, L. (2013). Suggestibility and memory conformity. In A.M. Ridley, F. Gabbert and D.J. La Rooy (eds), Suggestibility in legal contexts: Psychological research and forensic implications (pp 63-84). Oxford: Wiley Blackwell.

Gabbert, F., Hope, L., Carter, E., Boon, R and Fisher, R.P. (2016). The role of initial witness accounts within the investigative process (pp. 107-133). In G. Oxburgh, T. Myklebust, T. Grant and R. Milne (eds), Communication in investigative and legal contexts: Integrated approaches from forensic psychology, linguistics and law enforcement. Chichester: Wiley.

Gabbert, F., Hope, L. and Fisher, R.P. (2009). Protecting eyewitness evidence; Examining the efficacy of a self-administered interview tool. Law and Human Behavior, 33, 298-307.

Gabbert, F., Hope, L., Fisher, R.P. and Jamieson, K. (2012). Protecting against susceptibility to misinformation with a self-administered interview. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 568-75.

Gabbert, F., Memon, A. and Allan, K. (2003). Memory conformity: can eyewitnesses influence each other’s memories for an event? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 533-543.

Gable, P.A. and Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The blues broaden, but the nasty narrows attentional consequences of negative affects low and high in motivational intensity. Psychological Science, 21(2), 211-215.

Gable, P.A. and Harmon-Jones, E. (2012). Reducing attentional capture of emotion by broadening attention: increased global attention reduces early electrophysiological responses to negative stimuli. Biological Psychology, 90, 150-153.

Gais, S. and Born, J. (2004). Declarative memory consolidation: mechanisms acting during human sleep. Learning & Memory, 11, 679-685.

Gais, S., Albouy, G., Boly, M., Dang-Vu, T.T., Darsaud, A., Desseilles, M., Rauchs, G., Schabus, M., Sterpenich, V., Vandewalle, G., Maquet, P. and Peigneux, P. (2007). Sleep transforms the cerebral trace of declarative memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 18778-18783.

Galton, F. (1879). Psychometric experiments. Brain, 2, 149-162.

Gambles, J., Cameron, J., Day, S., Eastman, K., Hand, A., Hough, V., … Webb, D. (2007). RAC Report on Motoring 2007: Driving Safely? RAC.

Garland, D.J., Stein, E.S. and Muller, J.K. (1999). Air traffic controller memory: capabilities, limitations and volatility. In D.J. Garland and J.A. Wise (eds), Handbook of aviation human factors: Human factors in transportation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Gaskell, M.G. and Dumay, N. (2003). Lexical competition and the acquisition of novel words. Cognition, 89, 105-132.

Gates, N.J., Sachdev, P.S., Singh, M.A.F. and Valenzuela, M. (2011). Cognitive and memory training in adults at risk of dementia: a systematic review. BMC Geriatrics, 11. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-11-55.

Gathercole, S.E. and Baddeley, A.D. (1993). Working memory and language. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Gaver, W.W. (1986). Auditory icons: using sound in computer interfaces. Human-Computer Interaction, 2, 167-177.

Gaver, W.W. (1993). What in the world do we hear? An ecological approach to auditory event perception. Ecological Psychology, 5, 1-29.

Gawrylowicz, J., Memon, A. and Scoboria, A. (2013). Equipping witnesses with transferable skills: the self-administered interview. Psychology, Crime & Law, 20, 315-325.

Gazzaley, A., Cooney, J.W., McEvoy, K., Knight, R.T. and D’Esposito, M. (2005). Top-down enhancement and suppression of the magnitude and speed of neural activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 507-517.

Gegenfurtner, A., Lehtinen, E. and Saljo, R. (2011). Expertise differences in the comprehension of visualisations: a meta-analysis of eye-tracking research in professional domains. Educational Psychology Review, 23, 523-552.

Geiselman, R.E., Fisher, R.P., MacKinnon, D.P. and Holland, H.L. (1985). Eyewitness memory enhancement in the police interview: cognitive retrieval mnemonics versus hypnosis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 401-412.

George, A.M., Olesen, S. and Tait, R.J. (2013). Ecstasy use and depression: a 4-year longitudinal study among an Australian general community sample. Psychopharmacology, 229, 713-721.

Geretsegger, M., Elefant, C., Mössler, K.A. and Gold, C. (2014). Music therapy for people with autism spectrum disorder. The Cochrane Library.

Geukes, K., Mesagno, C., Hanrahan, S.J. and Kellmann, M. (2013). Activation of self-focus and self-presentation traits under private, mixed, and public pressure. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 35, 50-59.

Gevins, A., Smith, M.E., Leong, H., McEvoy, L., Whitfield, S., Du, R. and Rush, G. (1998). Monitoring working memory load during computer-based tasks with EEG pattern recognition methods. Human Factors, 40, 79-91.

Gibertini, M., Graham, C. and Cook, M.R. (1999). Self-report of circadian type reflects the phase of the melatonin rhythm. Biological Psychology, 50(1), 19-33.

Gibson, J.J. (1950). The perception of the visual world. Boston, MA: Houghton Miffin.

Gibson, J.J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Miffin.

Gibson, J.J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Gigerenzer, G. (2014). Risk savvy: How to make good decisions. New York: Penguin Books.

Gigerenzer, G. and Brighton, H. (2009). Homo heuristicus: why biased minds make better inferences. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 107-143.

Gigerenzer, G. and Gaissmaier, W. (2011). Heuristic decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 451-482.

Gigerenzer, G., Hertwig, R. and Pachur, T. (2011). Heuristics: The foundations of adaptive behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.

Gigerenzer, G. and Hoffrage, U. (1995). How to improve Bayesian reasoning without instruction: frequency formats. Psychological Review, 102, 684-704.

Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P.M. and the ABC Research Group. (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.

Gilovich, T., Griffin, D. and Kahneman, D. (eds). (2002). Heuristics and biases. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Gilovich, T., Vallone, R. and Tversky, A. (1985). The hot hand in basketball: on the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 295-314.

Gilovich, T., Wang, R.F., Regan, D. and Nishina, S. (2003). Regrets of action and inaction across cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 61-71.

Gingras, B., Honing, H., Peretz, I., Trainor, L.J. and Fisher, S.E. (2015). Defining the biological bases of individual differences in musicality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 370(1664), 20140092.

Glade, M.J. (2010). Caffeine - not just a stimulant. Nutrition, 26(10), 932-938.

Gladwell, M. (2005). Blink: The power of thinking without thinking. London: Penguin.

Glenberg, A.M. and Lehman T.S. (1980). Spacing repetitions over 1 week. Memory & Cognition, 8, 528-538.

Glisky, E.L. (2007). Changes in cognitive function in human aging. In D.R. Riddle (ed.), Brain aging models, methods, and mechanisms. Frontiers in Neuroscience. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Gluck, J. and Bluck, S. (2007). Looking back over the life span: a life story account of the reminiscence bump. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1928-1939.

Goel, N., Rao, H., Durmer, J.S. and Dinges, D.F. (2009). Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Seminars in Neurology, 29(4), 320-339.

Gold, D.R., Rogacz, S., Bock, N., Tosteson, T.D., Baum, T.M., Speizer, F.E. and Czeisler, C.A. (1992). Rotating shift work, sleep, and accidents related to sleepiness in hospital nurses. American Journal of Public Health, 82(7), 1011-1014.

Goldsmith, M., Koriat, A. and Pansky, A. (2005). Strategic regulation of grain size in memory reporting over time. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 505-525.

Goldstein, D.G., Johnson, E.J. and Sharpe, W.F. (2008). Choosing outcomes versus choosing products: consumer-focused retirement investment advice. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 440-456.

Golombek, D.A., Casiraghi, L.P., Agostino, P.V., Paladino, N., Duhart, J.M., Plano, S.A. and Chiesa, J.J. (2013). The times they’re a-changing: effects of circadian desynchronization on physiology and disease. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 107(4), 310-322.

Gonzalez-Diaz, J., Gossner, O. and Rogers, B.W. (2012). Performing best when it matters most: evidence from professional tennis. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 84, 767-781.

Goodale, M.A. and Milner, A.D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(1), 20-25.

Goodale, M.A. and Milner, A.D. (2006). One brain - two visual systems. The Psychologist, 19(11), 660-663.

Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, E., Dauman, J., Tuchtenhagen, F., Pelz, S., Becker, S., Kunert, H.J. et al. (2000). Impaired cognitive performance in drug free users of recreational ecstasy (MDMA). Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 68, 719-725.

Graham, H. (1994). Gender and class as dimensions of smoking behaviour in Britain: insights from a survey of mothers. Social Science and Medicine, 38 (5), 691-698.

Granhag, P.A. and Hartwig, M. (2015). The Strategic Use of Evidence (SUE) technique: a conceptual overview. In P.A. Granhag, A. Vrij and B. Verschuere (eds), Deception detection: Current challenges and cognitive approaches (pp. 231-251). Oxford: Wiley Blackwell.

Granier-Deferre, C., Bassereau, S., Ribeiro, A., Jacquet, A-Y. and DeCasper, A.J. (2011). A melodic contour repeatedly experienced by human near-term fetuses elicits a profound cardiac reaction one month after birth. PLOS ONE, 6(2), e17304.

Gray, R. (2004). Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: baseball batting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 42-54.

Green, H.J., Lemaire, P. and Dufau, S. (2007). Eye movement correlates of younger and older adults’ strategies for complex addition. Acta Psychologica, 125, 257-278.

Gregory, R.L. (1970). The intelligent eye. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.

Gregory, R.L. (1980). Perceptions as hypotheses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 290, 181-197.

Grey, S. and Mathews, A. (2000). Effects of training on interpretation of emotional ambiguity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(4), 1143-1162.

Griefahn, B. and Robens, S. (2010). The normalization of the cortisol awakening response and of the cortisol shift profile across consecutive night shifts - an experimental study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(10):1501-1509.

Griffiths, A. and Milne, R. (2005). Will it all end in tiers? Police interviews with suspects in Britain. In T. Williamson (ed), Investigative interviewing: Rights, research, regulation (pp. 167-189). Cullomp ton: Willan.

Griffiths, A., Milne, R. and Cherryman, J. (2011). A question of control? The formulation of suspect and witness interview question strategies by advanced interviewers. International Journal of Police Science and Management, 13, 1-13.

Griffiths, R.R. and Woodson, P.P. (1988). Caffeine and physical dependence: a review of human and laboratory animal studies. Psychopharmacology, 94, 437-451.

Groeger, J.A. (1997). Memory and remembering. Harlow: Longman.

Grohn, M., Lokki, T. and Takala, T. (2005). Comparison of auditory, visual and audio-visual navigation in a 3D space. ACM Transactions on Applied Perception, 2(4), 564-570.

Gronlund, S.D., Wixted, J.T. and Mickes, L. (2014). Evaluating eyewitness identification procedures using receiver operating characteristic analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(1), 3-10.

Groome D.H., with Brace, N., Edgar, G., Edgar, H., Eysenck, M.W., Manly, T., Ness, H., Pike, G., Scott, S. and Styles, E. (2014). An introduction to cognitive psychology: Processes and disorders (3rd edn). Hove: Psychology Press.

Groome, D. and Sterkaj, F. (2010). Retrieval-induced forgetting and clinical depression. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 63-70.

Groome, D. (1999). Memory. In D. Groome, H. Dewart, A. Esgate, K. Gurney, R. Kemp, and N. Towell (eds), An introduction to cognitive psychology: Processes and disorders. London: Psychology Press.

Gross, J., Hayne, H. and Drury, T. (2009). Drawing facilitates children’s reports of factual and narrative information: implications for educational contexts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 953-971.

Gruneberg, M.M. (1978). The feeling of knowing: memory blocks and memory aids. In M.M. Gruneberg and P.E. Morris (eds), Aspects of memory. London: Methuen.

Gruneberg, M.M. (1987). Linkword French, German, Spanish, Italian, Greek, Portuguese. London: Corgi.

Gruneberg, M.M. and Jacobs, G.C. (1991). In defence of linkword. The Language Learning Journal, 3, 25-29.

Gruneberg, M.M., Morris, P.E. and Sykes, R.N. (1991). The obituary on everyday memory and its practical applications is premature. American Psychologist, 46, 74-76.

Grüne-Yanoff, T. and Hertwig, R. (in press). Nudge versus boost: how coherent are policy and theory? Minds & Machines.

Guski, R. (1990). Auditory localization: effects of reflecting surfaces. Perception, 19, 819-830.

Gwizdka, J. and Chignell. M.H. (2004). Individual differences and task-based user interface evaluation: a case study of pending tasks in email. Interacting with Computers, 16, 769-797.

Haas, E.C. and Edworthy, J. (1996). Designing urgency into auditory warnings using pitch, speed and loudness. Computing and Control Engineering Journal, 7, 193-198.

Haber, R.N. and Myers, B.L. (1982). Memory for pictograms, pictures, and words separately and all mixed up. Perception, 11, 57-64.

Hagsand, A., Hjelmsäter, E.R.A., Granhag, P.A., Fahlke, C. and Söderpalm-Gordh, A. (2013). Bottled memories: on how alcohol affects eyewitness recall. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 54(3), 188-195.

Hallion, L.S. and Ruscio, A.M. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychological Bulletin, 137(6): 940-958.

Halpern, A.R. and Bartlett, J.C. (2011). The persistence of musical memories: a descriptive study of earworms. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 28(4), 425-432.

Halpern, A.R. and Müllensiefen, D. (2008). Effects of timbre and tempo change on memory for music. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(9), 1371-1384.

Hambrick, D.Z. and Tucker-Drob, E.M. (2014). The genetics of music accomplishment: evidence for gene-environment correlation and interaction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(1), 112-120.

Hameleers, P.A.H.M., Van Boxtel, M.P.J., Hogervorst, E., Riedel, W.J., Houx, P.J., Buntinx, F. and Jolles, J. (2000). Habitual caffeine consumption and its relation to memory, attention, planning capacity and psychomotor performance across multiple age groups. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 15, 573-581.

Hammond, K.R. (1996). Human judgment and social policy: Irreducible uncertainty, inevitable error, unavoidable injustice. New York: Oxford University Press.

Hampson, E. and Kimura, D. (1988). Reciprocal effects of hormone fluctuations on human perceptual and motor skills. Behavioural Neuroscience, 102(3), 456-459.

Hampson, E. (1990). Variations in sex-related cognitive abilities across the menstrual cycle. Brain and Cognition, 14(1), 26-43.

Han, D.H., Kim, B.N., Cheong, J.H., Kang, K.D. and Renshaw, P.F. (2014). Anxiety and attention shifting in professional baseball players. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 35, 708-713.

Han, D.H., Park, H.W., Kee, B.S., Na, C., Na, D.-H.E. and Zaichkowsky, L. (2011). Performance enhancement with low stress and anxiety modulated by cognitive flexibility. Psychiatry Investigations, 8, 221-226.

Hancock, P.J., Bruce, V. and Burton, A.M. (2000). Recognition of unfamiliar faces. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(9), 330-337.

Handel, S. (1989). Listening: An introduction to the perception of auditory events. Boston, MA: MIT Press.

Hansen, J. (2001). Increased breast cancer risk among women who work predominantly at night. Epidemiology, 12(1), 74-77

Hanton, S., Mellalieu, S.D. and Hall, R. (2004). Self-confidence and anxiety interpretation: a qualitative investigation. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 5, 477-495.

Harris, J.D. and Sergeant, R.L. (1971). Monaural/binaural minimum audible angles for a moving sound source. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 618-629.

Hartley, L.R., Lyons, D. and Dunne, M. (1987). Memory and the menstrual cycle. Ergonomics, 30(1), 111-120.

Hartwig, M., Granhag, P.A., Strömwall, L.A. and Kronkvist, O. (2006). Strategic us of evidence during police interviews: when training to detect deception works. Law and Human Behavior, 30, 603-619.

Harvey, A.J. (2015). When alcohol narrows the field of focal attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1-9.

Hasher, L., Zacks, R.T. and May, C.P. (1999). Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In D. Gopher and A. Koriat (eds), Attention & performance XVII. Cognitive regulation of performance: Interaction of theory and application (pp. 653-675). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Hashtroudi, S., Johnson, M.K., Vnek, N. and Ferguson, S.A. (1994). Aging and the effects of affective and factual focus on source monitoring and recall. Psychology and Aging, 9, 160-170.

Haslam, C. and Cook, M. (2002). Striking a chord with amnesic patients: evidence that song facilitates memory. Neurocase, 8(6), 453-465.

Hastie, R. and Dawes, R.M. (2001). Rational choice in an uncertain world. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Hatta, T. and Nagaya, K. (2009). Menstrual cycle phase effects on memory and Stroop task performance. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38(5), 821-827.

Hausmann, M., Slabbekoorn, D., Van Goozen, S.H.M., Cohen-Kettenis, P.T. and Guentuerkuen, O. (2000). Sex hormones affect spatial abilities during the menstrual cycle. Behavioural Neuroscience, 114(6), 1245-1250.

Hawkins, G, Hayes, B.K., Donkin, C., Pasqualino, M. and Newell, B.R. (2015). A Bayesian latent mixture model analysis shows that informative samples reduce base rate neglect. Decision 2, 306-318.

Haxby, J.V., Hoffman, E.A. and Gobbini, M.I. (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 223-233.

Hayes, B.K., Hawkins, G., Newell, B.R., Pasqualino, M. and Rehder, B. (2014). The role of causal models in multiple judgments under uncertainty. Cognition, 133, 611-620.

Hays, M.J., Kornell, N. and Bjork, R.A. (2013). When and why a failed test potentiates the effectiveness of subsequent study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 290-296.

Health and Social Care Information Centre (2010). Statistics on smoking, 2010. www.hscic.gov.uk/catalogue/PUB00684/smok-eng-2010-rep.pdf [accessed 2 June 2015].

Healy, D. (1987). Rhythm and blues: neurochemical, neuropharmacological and neuropsychological implications of a hypothesis of circadian rhythm dysfunction in affective disorders. Psychopharmacology, 93, 271-285.

Heathcote, D. (1994). The role of visuo-spatial working memory in the mental addition of multi-digit addends. Current Psychology of Cognition, 13, 207-245.

Hedden, T. (2007). Imaging cognition in the aging human brain. In D.R. Riddle (ed.). Brain aging models, methods, and mechanisms. Frontiers in Neuroscience. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Heffernan, T.M., Jarvis, H., Rodgers, J, Scholey, A.B. and Ling, J. (2001). Prospective memory, everyday cognitive failure and central executive function in recreational users of ecstasy. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 16, 607-612.

Heinzel, S., Schulte, S., Onken, J., Duong, Q-L., Riemer, T.G., Heinz, A., Kathmann, N. and Rapp, M.A. (2013). Working memory training improvements and gains in non-trained cognitive tasks in young and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 21, 146-173.

Heishman S.J., Kleykamp B.A. Singleton E.G. (2010). Meta-analysis of the acute effects of nicotine and smoking on human performance. Psycho pharmacology, 210, 453-469

Held, J.R., Riggs, M.L. and Dorman, C. (1999). The effect of prenatal cocaine exposure on neurobehavioural outcome: a meta-analysis. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 21(6), 619-625.

Hershkowitz, I., Lamb, M. and Katz, I. (2015). Does enhanced rapport-building alter the dynamics of investigative interviews with suspected victims of intra-familial abuse? Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 30, 6-14.

Hertel, P.T. (1992). Improving memory and mood through automatic and controlled procedures of mind. In D.J. Herrmann, H. Weingartner, A. Searleman and C.L. MacEvoy (eds), Memory improvement: Implications for memory theory. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Hertwig R., Benz, B. and Krauss, S. (2008). The conjunction fallacy and the many meanings of ‘and’. Cognition, 108, 740-753.

Hewitt, M. (2001). Nine force attrition study. London: The Association of Chief Police Officers of England, Wales, and Northern Ireland.

Higbee, K. (1977). Your memory: How it works and how to improve it. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Higbee, K. (2001). Your memory: How it works and how to improve it (2nd edn). New York: Marlowe.

Hill, D.M., Hanton, S., Matthews, N. and Fleming, S. (2010). Choking in sport: a review. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 3, 24-39.

Hilliar, K.F., Kemp, R.I. and Denson, T.F. (2010). Now everyone looks the same: alcohol intoxication reduces the own-race bias in face recognition. Law and Human Behavior, 34(5), 367-378.

Hills, B.L. (1980). Vision, visibility and perception in driving. Perception, 9, 183-216.

Hilton, D.J. (1995). The social context of reasoning: conversational inference and rational judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 248-271.

Hines, M. (2007). Do sex differences in cognition cause the shortage of women in science? In S.J. Ceci and Williams, W.M. (eds), Why aren’t more women in science?: Top researchers debate the evidence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Hitch, G.J. (1978). The role of short-term working memory in mental arithmetic. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 302-323.

Hitch, G.J. (1980). Developing the concept of working memory. In G. Claxton (ed), Cognitive psychology: New directions. London: Routledge Kegan Paul.

Ho, C.E. (1998). Letter recognition reveals pathways of second-order and third-order motion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(1), 400-404.

Ho, H-Z., Gilger, J.W. and Brink, T. (1986). Effects of menstrual cycle on spatial information processes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63, 743-751.

Hodgson, C. (2014). For the love of Radio 4. Chichester: Summersdale.

Hogarth, R.M. (2001). Educating intuition. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

Hogarth, R.M. (2010). Intuition: a challenge for psychological research on decision making. Psychological Inquiry, 21, 338-353.

Hogarth, R.M., Lejarraga, T. and Soyer, E. (in press). The two settings of kind and wicked environments. Current Directions in Psychological Science.

Hogarth, R.M. and Soyer, E. (2011). Sequentially simulated outcomes: kind experience versus nontransparent description. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 434-463.

Hogarth, R.M. and Soyer, E. (in press). Communicating forecasts: the simplicity of simulated experience. Journal of Business Research.

Hogervorst, E., Bandelow, S., Schmitt, J., Jentjens, R., Oliveira, M., Allgrove, J. and Gleeson, M. (2008). Caffeine improves physical and cognitive performance during exhaustive exercise. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 40(10), 1841-1851.

Holbrook, M.B. and Schindler, R.M. (1989). Some exploratory findings on the development of musical tastes. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(6),119-124.

Holliday, R., Humphries, J., Milne, R., Memon, A., Houlder, L., Lyons, A. and Bull, R. (2012). Reducing misinformation effects in older witnesses with Cognitive Interview mnemonics. Psychology & Aging, 27, 1191-1203.

Holst, V.F. and Pezdek, K. (1992). Scripts for typical crimes and their effects on memory for eyewitness testimony. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6, 573-587.

Hope, J.A. and Sherrill, J.M. (1987). Characteristics of skilled and unskilled mental calculators. Journal of Research in Mathematics Education, 18, 98-111.

Hope, L., Gabbert, F., Fisher, R.P. and Jamieson, K. (2014). Protecting and enhancing eyewitness memory: the impact of an initial recall attempt on performance in an investigative interview. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 304-313.

Hoppitt, L., Mathews, A., et al. (2010). Cognitive Mechanisms Underlying the Emotional Effects of Bias Modification. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(3), 312-325.

Horne, J.A. and Östberg, O. (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. International journal of Chronobiology, 4(2), 97-110.

Hornstein, S.L., Brown, A.S. and Mulligan, N.W. (2003). Long-term flashbulb memory for learning of Princess Diana’s death. Memory, 11, 293-306.

Horrey, W.J., Lesch, M.F. and Garabet, A. (2008). Assessing the awareness of performance decrements in distracted drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 40(2), 675-682.

Horswill, M.S., Helman, S., Ardiles, P. and Wann, J.P. (2005). Motorcycle accident risk could be inflated by a time to arrival illusion. Optometry & Vision Science, 82(8), 740-746.

Howard, C.Q., Maddern, A.J. and Privopoulos, E.P. (2011). Acoustic characteristics for effective ambulance sirens. Acoustics Australia, 39(2), 43-53.

Howard, J.H. and Howard, D.V. (1997). Learning and memory. In A.D. Fisk and W.A. Rogers (eds), Handbook of human factors and the older adult. San Diego: Academic Press.

Howe, M.L. and Courage, M.L. (1997). The emergence and early development of autobiographical memory. Psychological Review, 104, 499-523.

Howland, J., Rohsenow, D.J., Arnedt, J.T., Bliss, C.A., Hunt, S.K., Calise, T.V., Heeren, T., Winter, M., Littlefield, C. and Gottlieb, D.J. (2011). The acute effects of caffeinated versus noncaffeinated alcoholic beverage on driving performance and attention/reaction time. Addiction, 106(2), 335-341.

Hu, T.-Y., Xie, X. and Li, J. (2013). Negative or positive? The effect of emotion and mood on risky driving. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 16, 29-40.

Huang, X.D., Ariki, Y. and Jack, M.A. (1990). Hidden Markov models for speech recognition. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

Hubber, P.J., Gilmore, C. and Cragg, L. (2014). The roles of the central executive and visuospatial storage in mental arithmetic: a comparison across strategies. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 936-954.

Huguenard, B.R., Lerch, F.J., Junker, B.W., Patz, R.J. and Kass, R.E. (1997). Working memory failure in phone-based interaction. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction, 4, 67-102.

Hunt, L. and Bull, R. (2012). Differentiating genuine and false rape allegations. Psychiatry, Psychology and Law, 19, 682-691.

Hunter, J.M.L. (1979). Memory in everyday life. In M.M. Gruneberg and P.E. Morris (eds), Applied Problems in Memory. New York: Academic Press.

Huntsinger, J.R. (2012). Does positive affect broaden and negative affect narrow attentional scope? A new answer to an old question. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 595-600.

Hyman, I.E., Boss, S.M., Wise, B.M., McKenzie, K.E. and Caggiano, J.M. (2010). Did you see the unicycling clown? Inattentional blindness while walking and talking on a cell phone. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(5), 597-607.

Ikeda, H. and Hayashi, M. (2008). Effect of sleep inertia on switch cost and arousal level immediately after awakening from normal nocturnal sleep. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 6(2), 120-125.

Imbo, I. and LeFevre, J.A. (2010). The role of phonological and visual working memory in complex arithmetic for Chinese and Canadian educated adults. Memory & Cognition, 38, 176-185

Innes, M. (2003). The process structures of police homicide investigations. British Journal of Criminology, 42, 669-88.

Innocence Project (2015). Retrieved from: www.innocenceproject.org/news-events-exonerations/reevaluating-lineups-why-witnesses-make-mistakes-and-how-to-reduce-the-chance-of-a-isidentification.

Iso-Ahola, S.E. and Dotson, C.O. (2014). Psychological momentum: why success breeds success. Journal of General Psychology, 18, 19-33.

Istvan, J. and Matarazzo, J.D. (1984). Tobacco, alcohol and caffeine use: a review of their interrelationships. Psychological Bulletin, 95(2), 301-326.

Iversen, J.R., Repp, B.H. and Patel, A.D. (2009). Top-down control of rhythm perception modu lates early auditory responses. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169(1), 58-73.

Jaeggi, S.M., Buschkueh, M., Jonides, J. and Perrig, W.J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(19), 6829-6833.

Jagannath, A., Peirson, S.N. and Foster, R.G. (2013). Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in neuropsychiatric illness. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(5), 888-894.

Jain, A.K., Klare, B. and Park, U. (2012). Face matching and retrieval in forensics applications. IEEE MultiMedia, 19(1), 20-28.

James, F.O., Walker, C.D. and Boivin, D.B. (2004). Controlled exposure to light and darkness realigns the salivary cortisol rhythm in night shift workers. Chronobiology International, 21(6), 961-972.

James, J.E. (1994). Does caffeine enhance or merely restore degraded psychomotor performance? Neuropsychobiology, 30, 124-125.

James, J.E. (2014). Caffeine and cognitive performance: persistent methodological challenges in caffeine research. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 124C, 117-122.

Janata, P., Tillmann, B. and Bharucha, J.J. (2002). Listening to polyphonic music recruits domain-general attention and working memory circuits. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(2), 121-140.

Jäncke, L. (2008). Music, memory and emotion. Journal of Biology, 7(21.10), 1186.

Janssen, S.M., Rubin, D.C. and Conway, M.A. (2012). The reminiscence bump in the temporal distribution of the best football players of all time: Pelé, Cruijff or Maradona? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(1), 165-178.

Jenkins, J.J. (1985). Acoustic information for objects, places and events. In W.H. Warren and R.E. Shaw (eds), Persistence and Change: Proceedings of the First International Conference of Event Perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Jenkins, R. and Burton, A. M. (2008). 100% accuracy in automatic face recognition. Science, 319(5862), 435-435.

Jenkins, R., White, D., Van Montfort, X. and Burton, A.M. (2011). Variability in photos of the same face. Cognition, 121(3), 313-323.

Jenny, M.A., Pachur, T., Williams, S.L., Becker, E. and Margraf, J. (2013). Simple rules for detecting depression. Journal of Applied Research in Memory & Cognition, 2, 149-157.

Jerabek, I. and Standing, L. (1992). Imagined test situations produce contextual memory enhancement. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 400.

Jerrams-Smith, J. (2000). An intelligent human-computer interface for provision of on-line help. Artificial Intelligence Review, 14, 5-22.

Jerrams-Smith, J., Heathcote, D. and White, L. (1999). Working memory span as a usability factor in a virtual community of elderly people. In L. Brooks and C. Kimble (eds), Information systems: The next generation. Maidenhead: McGraw-Hill.

Jimura, K., Locke, H.S. and Braver, T.S. (2010). Prefrontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 8871-8876.

John, P., Cotterill, S., Moseley, A., Richardson L., Smith, G., Stoker, G. and Wales, C. (2011). Nudge, nudge, think, think: Experimenting with ways to change civic behaviour. London: Bloomsbury Academic.

Johnson, E.J. and Goldstein, D.G. (2003). Do defaults save lives? Science, 302, 1338-39.

Johnson, M.W., Strain, E.C. and Griffiths, R.R. (2010). Effects of oral caffeine pretreatment on response to intravenous nicotine and cocaine. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 18(4), 305-315.

Jones, R.A. (2001). Proust’s contribution to the psychology of memory: the Reminiscences from the standpoint of cognitive science. Theory and Psychology, 11, 255-271.

Jones, S.D. and Furner, S.M. (1989). The construction of audio icons and information cues for human-computer dialogues. In T. Megaw (ed) Con tem porary Ergonomics: Proceedings of the Ergo nomics Society’s 1989 Annual Conference. London: Taylor & Francis.

Joormann, J.P.T., Hertel, P., LeMoult, J. and Gotlib, I.H. (2009). Training forgetting of negative material in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 34-43.

Josselyn, S.A. and Frankland, P.W. (2012). Infantile amnesia: a neurogenic hypothesis. Learning and Memory, 19, 423-433.

Juliano, L.M., Fucito, L.M. and Harrell, P.T. (2011). the influence of nicotine dose and nicotine dose expectancy on the cognitive and subjective effects of cigarette smoking. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 19(2), 105-115.

Juslin, P., Nilsson, H. and Winman, A. (2009). Probability theory, not the very guide of life. Psychological Review, 116, 856-874.

Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Allen Lane.

Kahneman, D. and Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In T.D. Gilovich, D.W. Griffin and D. Kahneman (eds), Heuristics and biases (pp. 49-81). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Kahneman, D. and Klein, G.A. (2009). Conditions for intuitive expertise: a failure to disagree. American Psychologist, 64, 515-26.

Kahneman, D., Slovic, P. and Tversky, A. (eds). (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263-291.

Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J. and Chun, M.M. (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(11), 4302-4311.

Karatsoreos, I.N. (2014). Links between circadian rhythms and psychiatric disease. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience, 8, 162. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00162.

Karatsoreos, I.N., Bhagat, S., Bloss, E.B., Morrison, J.H. and McEwen, B.S. (2011). Disruption of circadian clocks has ramifications for metabolism, brain, and behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(4), 1657-1662.

Karbach, J. and Verhaeghen, P. (2014). Making working memory work: a Meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychological Science, 25(11), 2027-2037.

Karr, J.E., Areshenkoff, C.N., Rast, P.G-B. and Mauricio, A. (2014). An empirical comparison of the therapeutic benefits of physical exercise and cognitive training on the executive functions of older adults: a meta-analysis of controlled trials. Neuropsychology, 28, 829-845.

Kasper, R.W., Elliott, J.C. and Giesbrecht, B. (2012). Multiple measures of visual attention predict novice motor skill performance when attention is focused externally. Human Movement Science, 31, 1161-1174.

Kassin, S.M. (2008). False confessions: causes, consequences, and implications for reform. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(4), 249-253.

Kassin, S.M. (2012). Why confessions trump innocence. American Psychologist, 67, 431-445.

Kassin, S.M., Bogart, D. and Kerner, J. (2012). Confessions that corrupt: evidence from the DNA exoneration case files. Psychological Science, 23 (1), 41-45.

Kassin, S.M., Tubb, V.A., Hosch, H.M. and Memon, A. (2001). On the ‘general acceptance’ of eyewitness testimony research. American Psychologist, 56, 405-416.

Kaufmann, C., Weber, M. and Haisley, E. (2013). The role of experience sampling and graphical displays on one’s investment risk appetite. Management Science, 59, 323-340.

Kebbell, M., Hatton, C. and Johnson. S. (2004). Witnesses with intellectual disabilities in court: what questions are asked and what influence do they have? British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 98-102.

Kebbell, M. and Milne, R. (1998). Police officers’ perceptions of eyewitness performance in forensic investigations. Journal of Social Psychology, 138, 232-330.

Kebbell, M., Milne, R. and Wagstaff, G. (1999). The cognitive interview: a survey of its forensic effectiveness. Psychology, Crime and Law, 5, 101-116.

Kebbell, M. and Wagstaff, G. (1999). Face value? Evaluating the accuracy of eyewitness information. Police Research Series Paper 102. London: Home Office.

Kelemen, W.L. and Creeley, C.E. (2001). Caffeine (4mg/kg) influences sustained attention and delayed free recall but not memory predictions. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experi mental, 16, 309-319.

Kemp, R.I., Pike, G.E. and Brace, N.A. (2001). Video-based identification procedures: combining best practice and practical requirements when designing identification systems. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7(4), 802-807.

Kemp, R., Towell, N. and Pike, G. (1997). When seeing should not be believing: photographs, credit cards and fraud. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11(3), 211-222.

Kenny, I.C., Wallace, E.S. and Otto, S.R. (2008). Driving performance variability among elite golfers. Engineering of Sport 7(1), 387-395.

Kerr, J.S. and Hindmarch, I. (1991). Alcohol, cognitive function and psychomotor performance. Reviews on Environmental Health, 9, 117-122.

Kerr, J.S. and Hindmarch, I. (1998). The effects of alcohol alone or in combination with other drugs on information processing, task performance and subjective responses. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 13, 1-9.

Kerr, J.S., Sherwood, N. and Hindmarch, I. (1991). Separate and combined effects of the social drugs on psychomotor performance. Psycho pharmacology, 104, 113-119.

Keskinen, E., Ota, H. and Katila, A. (1998). Older drivers fail in intersections: speed discrepancies between older and younger male drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 30(3), 323-330.

Kiefer, R.J., Flannagan, C.A. and Jerome, C.J. (2006). Time-to-collision judgements under realistic driving conditions. Human Factors, 48(2), 334-345.

Kimura, D. (1996). Sex, sexual orientation and sex hormones influence human cognitive function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6, 259-263.

Kimura, D. and Hampson, E. (1994). Cognitive pattern in men and women is influenced by fluctuations in sex hormones. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(2), 57-61.

Kissling, E.A. (2006). Capitalising on the curse: the business of menstruation. Boulder, COL: Lynne Rienner.

Klaassen, E.B., de Groot, R.H., Evers, E.A., Snel, J., Veerman, E.C., Ligtenberg, A.J., Jolles, J. and Veltman, D.J. (2013). The effect of caffeine on working memory load-related brain activation in middle-aged males. Neuropharmacology, 64, 160-167.

Klatzkin, R.R., Mechlin, B. and Girdler, S.S. (2010). Menstrual cycle phase does not influence gender differences in experimental pain sensitivity. European Journal of Pain, 14(1), 77-82.

Klayman, J. (1995). Varieties of confirmation bias. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 32, 385-418

Klayman, J. (2001). Ambivalence in (not about) naturalistic decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 14, 372-73.

Klein, G.A. (1993). A recognition-primed decision (RPD) model of rapid decision making. In G.A. Klein, J. Orasanu, R. Calderwood and C.E. Zsambok (eds), Decision making in action: Models and methods. Norwood, CT: Ablex.

Klein, G.A. (1998). Sources of power: How people make decisions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Klein, G.A. (2009). Streetlights and shadows: Searching for the keys to adaptive decision making. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Kleinberg, K.F., Vanezis, P. and Burton, A.M. (2007). Failure of anthropometry as a facial identification technique using high-quality photographs. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 52(4), 779-783.

Klostermann, A., Kredel, R. and Hossner, E.-J. (2013). The ‘quiet eye’ and motor performance: task demands matter! Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1270-1278.

Koedijker, J.M., Poolton, J.M., Maxwell, J.P., Oudejans, R.D.D., Beek, P.J. and Masters, R.S.W. (2011). Attention and time constraints in perceptual-motor learning and performance: instruction, analogy, and skill level. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 245-256.

Koelega, H.S. (1998). Effects of caffeine, nicotine and alcohol on vigilance performance. In J. Snel and M.M. Lorist (eds), Nicotine, caffeine and social drinking: Behaviour and brain function. Amsterdam: Harwood Academic Publishers.

Köhnken, G., Milne, R., Memon, A. and Bull, R. (1999). The cognitive interview: a meta-analysis. Psychology, Crime & Law, 5, 3-28.

Kolla, B.P. and Auger, R.R. (2011). Jet lag and shift work sleep disorders: how to help reset the internal clock. Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine, 78(10), 675-684.

Konishi, K., Kumashiro, M., Izumi, H. and Higuchi, Y. (2008). Effects of the menstrual cycle on working memory: comparison of postmenstrual and premenstrual phases. Industrial Health, 46(3), 253-260. http://doi.org/10.2486/indhealth.46.253

Kopiez, R. and In Lee, J. (2008). Towards a general model of skills involved in sight reading music. Music Education Research, 10(1), 41-62.

Koppel, J. and Berntsen, D. (2015). The peaks of life: the differential temporal locations of the reminiscence bump across disparate cueing methods. Journal of Applied Research in Memory & Cognition, 4(1), 66-80.

Koriat, A. and Goldsmith, M. (1996). Memory metaphors and the real-life/laboratory controversy: correspondence versus storehouse conceptions of memory. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 19, 167-188.

Kott, J., Leach, G. and Yan, L. (2012). Direction-dependent effects of chronic ‘jet-lag’ on hippocampal neurogenesis. Neuroscience Letters, 515 (2), 177-180.

Kouneiher, F., Charron, S. and Koechlin, E. (2009). Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 939-946.

Kramer, G., Walker, B., Bonebright, T., Cook, P. and Flowers, J.H. (2010). Sonification report: status of the field and research agenda. Faculty Publications. Department of Psychology, Paper 44. http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/psychfacpub/444 [accessed 4 December 2015].

Kramer, H.T., Buckhout, R. and Euginio, P. (1990). Weapon focus, arousal and eyewitness memory. Law and Human Behavior, 14, 167-184.

Króliczak, G., Heard, P., Goodale, M.A. and Gregory, R.L. (2006). Dissociation of perception and action unmasked by the hollow-face illusion. Brain Research, 1080, 9-16.

Kruger, J. and Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1121-1134.

Krumhansl, C.L. (2010). Plink: ‘thin slices’ of music. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27(5), 337-354.

Krumhansl, C.L. and Zupnick, J.A. (2013). Cascading reminiscence bumps in popular music. Psychological Science, 24(10), 2057-2068.

Krynski, T.R. and Tenenbaum, J.B. (2007). The role of causality in judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 430-450.

Kubovy, M. and Van Valkenburg, D. (2001). Auditory and visual objects. Cognition, 80, 97-126.

Künzel, H. (1994). On the problem of speaker identification by victims and witnesses. Forensic Linguistics, 1, 45-57.

Kvavilashvili, L. and Ellis, J. (2004). Ecological validity and the real life/laboratory controversy in memory research: a critical and historical review. History and Philosophy of Psychology, 6, 59-80.

Kyle, J., Fox, H.C. and Whalley, L.J. (2010). Caffeine, cognition, and socioeconomic status. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 20, 151-159.

Kyllonen, P.C. (1996). Is working memory capacity Spearman’s g? In I. Dennis and P. Tapsfield (eds), Human abilities: Their nature and measurement. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Lahtinen, V., Lonka, K. and Lindblom-Ylanne, K. (1997). Spontaneous study strategies and the quality of knowledge construction. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 13-24.

Lamberty, G.J., Beckwith, B.E. and Petros, T.V. (1990). Posttrial treatment with ethanol enhances recall of prose narratives. Physiology and Behaviour, 48(5), 653-658.

Landauer, T.K. and Bjork, R.A. (1978). Optimal rehearsal patterns and name learning. In M.M. Gruneberg, P.E. Morris and R.N. Sykes (eds), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 625-632), London: Academic Press.

Lane, J.D. and Rose, J.E. (1995). Effects of daily caffeine intake on smoking behaviour in the natural environment. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 3, 49-55.

Langham, M., Hole, G., Edwards, J. and O’Neil, C. (2002). An analysis of ‘looked but failed to see’ accidents involving parked police vehicles. Ergonomics, 45(3), 167-185.

Larrick, R.P. (2004). Debiasing. In D.J. Koehler and N. Harvey (eds), The Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (pp. 316-337). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Larsson, A.S., Granhag, P.A. and Spjut, E. (2003). Children’s recall and the cognitive interview: do the positive effects hold over time? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 2203-214.

Laughery, K.R. and Fowler, R.H. (1980). Sketch artist and identi-kit procedures for recalling faces. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65(3), 307-316.

Law, R., Groome, D., Thorn, L., Potts, R. and Buchanan, T. (2012). The relationship between retrieval-induced forgetting, anxiety, and personality. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 25, 711-718.

Law, R., Hucklebridge, F., Thorn, L., Evans, P. and Clow, A. (2013). State variation in the cortisol awakening response. Stress, 16 (5), 483-492.

Lazarus, R.S. (1982). Thoughts on the relationship between emotion and cognition. American Psychologist, 37, 1019-1024.

Lazarus, R.S. and Opton, E.M.J. (1966). The study of psychological stress: a summary of theoretical formulations and experimental findings. In C.D. Spielberger (ed), Anxiety and behaviour (pp. 225-262). New York: Academic Press.

Le Houezec, J. (1998). Pharmacokinetics and pharmaco dynamics of nicotine. In J. Snel and M.M. Lorist (eds), Nicotine, caffeine and social drinking: Behaviour and brain function. Amsterdam: Harwood Academic Publishers.

Lee, D.N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception, 5(4), 437-459.

Lee, H., Roh, S. and Kim, D.J. (2009). Alcohol-induced blackout. International Journal of Environ mental Research and Public Health, 6(11), 2783-2792.

Lee, T.G. and Grafton, S.T. (2015). Out of control: diminished prefrontal activity coincides with impaired motor performance due to choking under pressure. NeuroImage, 105, 145-155.

Lee, T.-H., Baek, J., Lu, Z.-L. and Mather, M. (2014). How arousal modulates the visual contrast sensitivity function. Emotion, 14(5), 978-984.

Leeney, D.G. and Muller-Johnson, K. (2011). Examining the forensic quality of police call-centre interviews. Psychology, Crime and Law, 18, 669-688.

Lehrer, J. (2009). The decisive moment: How the brain makes up its mind. Melbourne: Text Publishing.

Leippe, M.R. (1995). The case for expert testimony about eyewitness memory. Psychology, Public Policy and Law, 1(4), 909-959.

Lemaire, P., Abdi, H. and Fayol, M. (1996). The role of working memory resources in simple cognitive arithmetic. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 8, 73-103.

Leplâtre, G. and Brewster, S.A. (1998). An investigation of using music to provide navigation cues. In S.A. Brewster and A.D.N. Edwards (eds), Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Auditory Display. Glasgow: British Computer Society.

Leplâtre, G. and Brewster, S.A. (2000). Designing non-speech sounds to support navigation in mobile phone menus. In P.R. Cook (ed) Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Auditory Display. Atlanta, GA: International Community for Auditory Display.

LePort, A.K.R., Mattfield, A.T., Dickenson-Anson, H. Fallon, J.H., Stark, C.E.L., Kruggel, F., Cahill, L. and McGaugh, J.L. (2012). Behavioural and Neuroanatomical investigation of highly superior autobiographical memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 98, 78-92.

Lerdahl, F. and Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Leroi, I., Sheppard, J.M. and Lyketsos, C.G. (2002). Cognitive function after 11.5 years of alcohol use: relation to alcohol use. American Journal of Epidemiology, 156(8), 747-752.

Levin, E.D. (1992). Nicotinic systems and cognitive function. Psychopharmacology, 108, 417-431.

Levitin, D.J. (1994). Absolute memory for musical pitch: evidence from the production of learned melodies. Perception & Psychophysics, 56(4), 414-423.

Levitin, D.J. (2012). What does it mean to be musical? Neuron, 73(4), 633-637.

Levitt, S.D. and List, J.A. (2008). Homo Economicus evolves. Science, 319, 909-10.

Li, X., Logan, R.J. and Pastore, R.E. (1991). Perception of acoustic source characteristics: walking sounds. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 90, 3036-3049.

Liberman, A.M. (1970). The grammars of speech and language. Cognitive Psychology, 1, 301-323.

Liberman. A.M., Cooper, H.S., Shankweiler, D.S. and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychophysical Review, 74, 431-461.

Lichtenstein, S., Slovic, P., Fischoff, B., Layman, M. and Coombs, B. (1978). Judged frequency of lethal events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 551-578.

Lieberman, H.R. (1992). Caffeine. In A.P. Smith and D.M. Jones (eds), Handbook of human performance (vol. 2, pp. 49-72). London: Academic Press.

Lieberman, H.R., Tharion, W.J., Shukitt-Hale, B., Speckman, K.L. and Tulley, R. (2002). Effects of caffeine, sleep loss, and stress on cognitive performance and mood during US Navy SEAL training. Psychopharmacology, 164(3), 250-261.

Lieberman, H.R., Wurtman, R.J., Emde, G.G., Roberts, C. and Coviella, I.L.G. (1987). The effects of low doses of caffeine on human performance and mood. Psychopharmacology, 92, 308-312.

Lindenberger, U., Li, S., Gruber, W. and Müller, V. (2009), Brains swinging in concert: cortical phase synchronization while playing guitar. BMC Neuroscience, 10(22).

Lindsay, D.S. (2008). Source monitoring. In H.L. Roediger (ed), Cognitive Psychology of Memory (vol. 2, pp. 325-348). Oxford: Elsevier.

Lindsay, R.C., Ross, D.F., Read, J.D. and Toglia, M.P. (eds). (2007). Handbook of eyewitness psychology: Vol. II: Memory for people. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

Lindsay, R.C. and Wells, G.L. (1985). Improving eyewitness identifications from lineups: simultaneous versus sequential lineup presentation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70(3), 556-564.

Lingwood, J. and Bull, R. (2013). Interviewing young adolescent suspects: when to reveal incriminating information? The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context, 5, 141-146.

Linton, M. (1975). Memory for real-world events. In D.A. Norman and D.A. Rumelhart (eds), Explorations in cognition. San Francisco: Freeman.

Lippa, R.A., Collaer, M.L. and Peters, M. (2010). Sex differences in mental rotation and line angle judgements are positively associated with gender equality and economic development across 53 nations. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39, 990-997.

Lippmann, R.R. (1989). Review of neural networks for speech recognition. Neural Computation, 1, 1-38.

Lippmann, R.R. (1997). Speech recognition by machines and humans. Speech Communication, 22, 1-15.

Lipshitz, R., Klein, G., Orasanu, J. and Salas, E. (2001). Taking stock of naturalistic decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 14, 331-352.

Lisdahl, K.M., Gilbart, E.R., Wright, N.E. and Shollenbarger, S. (2013). Dare to delay? The impact of adolescent alcohol and marijuana use onset on cognition, brain structure and function. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4, 53.

Lisker, L. and Abramson, A.S. (1970). The voicing dimension: some experiments in comparative phonetics. In Proceedings of the 6th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Prague: Academia.

Locke, S. and Kellar, L. (1973). Categorical perception in a non-linguistic mode. Cortex, 9(4), 355-369.

Lockhart, R.S. and Craik, F.I.M. (1990). Levels of processing: a retrospective commentary on a framework for memory research. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 44, 87-112.

Loft, S., Sanderson, P., Neal, A. and Mooij, M. (2007). Modeling and predicting mental work-load in en route air traffic control: critical review and broader implications. Human Factors, 49, 376-399.

Loftus, E.F. (1975). Leading questions and the eye witness report. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 560-572.

Loftus, E.F. (1979). Eyewitness Testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Loftus, E.F. (2013). 25 years of eyewitness science … finally pays off. Perspectives on Psycho logical Science, 8(5), 556-557.

Loftus, E.F. and Burns, T. (1982). Mental shock can produce retrograde amnesia. Memory & Cognition, 10, 318-323.

Loftus, E.F. and Greene, E. (1980). Warning: even memory for faces may be contagious. Law and Human Behaviour, 4, 323-334.

Loftus, E.F., Loftus, G.R. and Messo, J. (1987). Some facts about weapon focus. Law and Human Behavior, 11, 55-62.

Loftus, E.F., Miller, D.G. and Burns, H.J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 19-31.

Loftus, E.F. and Palmer, J.C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: an example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 13, 585-589.

Loftus, E.F. and Pickrell, J.E. (1995). The formation of false memories. Psychiatric Annals, 25, 720-725.

Loftus, E.F. and Zanni, G. (1975). Eyewitness testimony: the influence of the wording of a question. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 5, 866-888.

Logan, G.D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492-527.

Logie, R.H. (1993). Working memory and human-machine systems. In J.A. Wise, V.D. Hopkin and P. Stager (eds), Verification and validation of complex systems: Human factors issues. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Logie, R.H. and Baddeley, A.D. (1987). Cognitive processes in counting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13, 310-326.

Logie, R.H., Gilhooly, K.J. and Wynn, V. (1994). Counting on working memory in arithmetic prob lem solving. Memory & Cognition, 22, 395-410.

Logie, R.H., Horne, M.J. and Pettit, L.D. (2015). When cognitive performance does not decline across the lifespan. In R.H. Logie and R.G. Morris (eds), Working memory and ageing (pp. 21-47). Hove: Psychology Press.

Logothesis, N.K. (1994). Physiological studies of motion inputs. In A.T. Smith (ed), Visual detection of motion (pp. 177-216). London: Academic Press.

Loke, W.H. (1993). Caffeine and automaticity in encoding prelexical tasks: theory and some data. Human Psychopharmacology, 8, 77-95.

Loke, W.H., Hinrichs, J.V. and Ghoneim, M.M. (1985). Caffeine and diazepam: separate and combined effects on mood, memory and psychomotor performance. Psychopharmacology, 87, 344-350.

Lorayne, H. and Lucas, J. (1974). The memory book. London: W.H. Allen.

Lotto, A. and Holt, L. (2010). Psychology of auditory perception. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(5), 449-593.

Lovallo, D. and Kahneman, D. (2003). Delusions of success. Harvard Business Review, 81, 57-63.

Loveday, C. and Conway, M.A. (in prep) Remembering and imagining music.

Loveday, C., Eardley, E., Edginton, T. and Conway, M.A. (in prep.) Factors influencing the musical reminiscence bump.

Lucas, P. (1994). An evaluation of the communicative ability of auditory icons and earcons. In G. Kramer and S. Smith (eds), Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Auditory Display. Santa Fe, NM: Addison-Wesley.

Luoma, J. (1988). Drivers’ eye fixations and perceptions. In A. G. Gale, M. H. Freeman, C. M. Haslegrave, P. Smith and S. P. Taylor (eds), Vision in Vehicles II (pp. 231-237). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

Luoma, J., Schumann, J. and Traube, E.C. (1996). Effects of retroreflector positioning on nighttime recognition of pedestrians. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 28(3), 377-383.

Luria, A.R. (1975). The mind of a mnemonist. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

Lustig, C., May, C.P. and Hasher, L. (2001). Working memory span and the role of proactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 199-207.

Luus, C.A. and Wells, G.L. (1994). The malleability of eyewitness confidence: co-witness and perseverance effects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(5), 714.

Maass, A. and Köhnken, G. (1989). Eyewitness identification: simulating the ‘weapon effect’. Law and Human Behavior, 13(4), 397-408.

McAllister, H.A., Bregman, N.J. and Lipscombe, T.J. (1988). Speed estimates by eyewitnesses and earwitnesses: how vulnerable to post-event information? Journal of General Psychology, 115, 25-35.

McClean, J.F. and Hitch, G.J. (1999). Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 240-260.

McDaniel, M.A. and Fisher, R.P. (1991). Tests and test feedback as learning sources. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 16, 192-201.

McDaniel, M.A., Roediger, H.L.III and McDermott, K.B. (2007). Generalising test-enhanced learning from the laboratory to the classroom. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 200-206.

MacDonald, S., Snook, B. and Milne, R. (in press). Witness interview training: a field evaluation. Journal of Police and Criminological Psychology.

MacDonald, S., Uesiliana, K. and Hayne, H. (2000). Cross-cultural and gender differences in childhood amnesia. Memory, 8, 365-376.

McDougal, S.J.P. and Gruneberg, M. (2002). What memory strategy is best for examinations in psychology? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 451-458.

McDougall, A. and Bull, R. (2015). Detecting truth in suspect interviews: the effect of use of evidence (early and gradual) and time delay on criteria-based content analysis, reality monitoring and inconsistency within suspect statements. Psychol ogy, Crime & Law, 21, 514-530.

McEvoy, S.P., Stevenson, M.R., McCartt, A.T., Woodward, M., Haworth, C., Palamara, P. and Cercarelli, R. (2005). Role of mobile phones in motor vehicle crashes resulting in hospital attendance: a case-crossover study. BMJ, 331(7514), 428.

McEvoy, S.P., Stevenson, M.R. and Woodward, M. (2007). The prevalence of, and factors associated with, serious crashes involving a distracting activity. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 39(3), 475-482.

McGookin, D. and Brewster, S. (2011). Earcons. In T. Hermann, A. Hunt and J.G. Neuhoff (eds), The sonification handbook. Berlin: Logos.

McGurk, H. and MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264, 746-748.

Machi, M.S., Staum, M., Callaway, C.W., Moore, C., Jeong, K., Suyama, J. and Hostler, D. (2012). The relationship between shift work, sleep, and cognition in career emergency physicians. Aca demic Emergency Medicine, 19(1), 85-91.

McKenzie, C.R.M. (2005). Judgment and decision making. In K. Lamberts and R. Goldstone (eds), Handbook of cognition (pp. 321-338). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Mackintosh, B., Mathews, A., et al. (2006). Induced biases in emotional interpretation influence stress vulnerability and endure despite changes in context. Behavior Therapy, 37(3), 209-222.

Mackworth, N.H. (1948). The breakdown of vigilance during prolonged visual search. Quar terly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1, 6-21.

MacLeod, C. and Clarke, P.J.F. (2013). Cognitive bias modification: a new frontier in cognition and emotion research. In M.D. Robinson, E.R. Watkins and E. Harmon-Jones (eds), Handbook of cognition and emotion. New York: Guilford Press.

MacLeod, C., Mathews, A. and Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1): 15-20.

MacLeod, C.M. (2010). When learning met memory. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(4), 227-240.

MacLeod, M.D. (2000). The future is always brighter: temporal orientation and psychological adjustment to trauma. In V. Violanti, D. Paton and C. Dunning (eds), Alternative approaches to debriefing (pp. 166-186), Springfield IL: Charles C. Thomas.

MacLeod, M.D. and Macrae, C.N. (2001). Gone today but here tomorrow: the transient nature of retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 12, 148-152.

MacLeod, M.D. and Saunders, J. (2005). The role of inhibitory control in the production of misinformation effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 31, 964-979.

McLeod, R.W. and Ross, H.E. (1983). Optic-flow and cognitive factors in time-to-collision estimates. Perception, 12(4), 417-423.

Macrae, C.N. and Bodenhausen, G.V. (2001). Social cognition: categorical person perception. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 239-256.

Macrae, C.N. and Macleod, M.D. (1999). On recollections lost: when practice makes imperfect. Journal of Personality and Social psychology, 77, 463-473.

Magazzù, D., Comelli, M. and Marinoni, A. (2006). Are car drivers holding a motorcycle licence less responsible for motorcycle-car crash occurrence?: a non-parametric approach. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 38(2), 365-370.

Maguire, E.A., Henson, R.N.A., Mummery, C.J. and Frith, C.D. (2001). Activity in prefrontal cortex, not hippocampus, varies parametrically with the increasing remoteness of memories. Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neurosciences Research, 12, 441-444.

Maguire, E.A., Valentine, E.R., Wilding, J.M. and Kapur, N. (2003). Routes to remembering: the brains behind superior memory. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 90-95.

Maguire, M.S. and Byth, W. (1998). The McCollough Effect across the menstrual cycle. Perception and Psychophysics, 60(2), 221-226.

Mahon, P.T. (1981). Report of the Royal Commission to inquire into the crash on Mount Erebus of a DC10 aircraft operated by Air New Zealand Limited.

Makous, J.C. and Middlebrooks, J.C. (1990). Two dimensional sound localisation by human listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 87, 2188-2200.

Malpass, R.S. (2006). A policy evaluation of simultaneous and sequential lineups. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 12(4), 394-418.

Malpass, R.S. and Devine, P.G. (1981). Eyewitness identification: lineup instructions and the absence of the offender. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66(4), 482-489.

Mantyla, T. (1986). Optimising cue effectiveness: recall of 500 and 600 incidentally learned words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 66-71.

Marczinski, C.A and Fillmore, M.T. (2006). Clubgoers and their trendy cocktails: implications of mixing caffeine into alcohol on information processing and subjective reports of intoxication. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 14(4), 450-458.

Marquié, J.C., Tucker, P., Folkard, S., Gentil, C. and Ansiau, D. (2014). Chronic effects of shift work on cognition: findings from the VISAT longitudinal study. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 72(4), 258-264.

Martignon, L., Vitouch, O., Takezawa, M. and Forster, M. (2003). Naïve and yet enlightened: from natural frequencies to fast and frugal decision trees. In D. Hardman and L. Macchi (eds), Thinking: Psychological perspectives on reasoning, judgment, and decision making (pp. 189-211). Chichester: Wiley.

Martire, K.A. and Kemp, R.I. (2009). The impact of eyewitness expert evidence and judicial instruction on juror ability to evaluate eyewitness testimony. Law and Human Behavior, 33(3), 225-236.

Martire, K.A. and Kemp, R.I. (2011). Can experts help jurors to evaluate eyewitness evidence? A review of eyewitness expert effects. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 16(1), 24-36.

Marván, M.L., Vázquez-Toboada, R. and Chrisler, J.C. (2014). Ambivalent sexism, attitudes towards menstruation and menstrual cycle-related symptoms. International Journal of Psychology, 49(4), 280-287. http://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12028

Marzano, C., Ferrara, M., Moroni, F. and De Gennaro, L. (2011). Electroencephalographic sleep inertia of the awakening brain. Neuroscience, 176, 308-317.

Massen, C., Vaterrodt-Plunnecke, B., Krings, L. and Hilbig, B.E. (2009). Effects of instruction on learners’ ability to generate an effective pathway in the method of loci. Memory, 17, 724-731.

Masters, R.S.W. (1992). Knowledge, (k)nerves and know-how: the role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 343-358.

Masters, R.S.W. and Maxwell, J. (2004). Implicit motor learning, reinvestment and movement disruption: what you don’t know won’t hurt you? In A.M. Williams and N.J. Hodges (eds), Skill acquisition in sport: Research, theory and practice. London: Routledge.

Masters, R.S.W. and Maxwell, J. (2008). The theory of reinvestment. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 160-183.

Masterton, B. and Diamond, I.T. (1973). Hearing: central neural mechanisms. In E.C. Carterette and M.P. Friedman (eds), Handbook of perception (vol. 3). New York: Academic Press.

Mathews, A. (1990). Why worry? The cognitive function of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(6), 455-468.

Mathews, A. and Mackintosh, B. (2000). Induced emotional interpretation bias and Anxiety. Jour nal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(4), 602-615.

Mathews, A. and MacLeod, C. (2002). Induced processing biases have causal effects on anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 16(2), 331-354.

Mathews, A., Mogg, K. et al. (1995). Effect of psychological treatment on cognitive bias in generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 293-303.

Mathews, A., Yiend, J. and Lawrence, A.D. (2004). Individual differences in the modulation of fear-related brain activation by attentional control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(10), 1683-1694.

Mathews, G., Davies, D.R., Westerman, S.J. and Stammers, R.B. (2000). Human performance: Cognition, stress and individual differences. Hove: Psychology Press.

Mathur, A., Vijayakumar, S.H., Chakrabarti, B. and Singh, N.C. (2015). Emotional responses to Hindu stani raga music: the role of musical structure. Frontiers in Psychology, 6.

Mattison, M.L., Dando, C.J. and Ormerod, T. (In press). Sketching to remember: episodic free recall task support for child witnesses and victims with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

Measham, F. (2006). The new policy mix: alcohol, harm minimisation and determined drunkenness in contemporary society. International Journal of Drug Policy, 17, 258-26.

Mecklenburg, S.H., Bailey, P.J. and Larson, M.R. (2008). The Illinois field study: a significant contribution to understanding real world eyewitness identification issues. Law and Human Behavior, 32(1), 22-27.

Meehl, P.E. (1986). Causes and effects of my disturbing little book. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 370-375.

Megreya, A.M. and Burton, A.M. (2006). Unfamiliar faces are not faces: evidence from a matching task. Memory & Cognition, 34, 865-876.

Megreya, A.M. and Burton, A.M. (2007). Hits and false positives in face matching: a familiarity-based dissociation. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(7), 1175-1184.

Megreya, A.M. and Burton, A.M. (2008). Matching faces to photographs: poor performance in eyewitness memory (without the memory). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14(4), 364-372.

Megreya, A.M., Sandford, A. and Burton, A.M. (2013). Matching face images taken on the same day or months apart: the limitations of photo ID. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27(6), 700-706.

Megreya, A.M., White, D. and Burton, A.M. (2011). The other-race effect does not rely on memory: evidence from a matching task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(8), 1473-1483.

Meijer, P.B.L. (1993). An experimental system for auditory image representations. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 39, 112-121 (reprinted in the 1993 IMIA Yearbook of Medical Informatics).

Meissner, C. (2002). Applied aspects of the instructional bias effect in verbal overshadowing. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 295-305.

Meissner, C.A. and Brigham, J.C. (2001). Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: a meta-analytic review. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7(1), 3.

Meissner, C.A., Susa, K.J. and Ross, A.B. (2013). Can I see your passport please? Perceptual discrimination of own-and other-race faces. Visual Cognition, 21(9-10), 1287-1305.

Melby-Lervåg, M. and Hulme, C. (2012). Is Working Memory Training Effective? A Meta-Analytic Review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270-291.

Mellalieu, S.D., Neil, R. and Hanton, S. (2006). Self-confidence as a mediator of the relationship between competitive anxiety intensity and interpretation. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 77, 263-270.

Melnick, S.M., Kubie, J.L., Laungani, R. and Dow-Edwards, D.L. (2001). Impairment of spatial learning following preweaning cocaine exposure in the adult rat. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 23(5), 445-451.

Memmert, D. (2009). Pay attention! A review of visual attentional expertise in sport. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 2, 119-138.

Memon, A., Bull, R. and Smith, M. (1995). Improving the quality of the police interview: can training in the use of cognitive techniques help? Policing and Society, 5, 53-68.

Memon, A., Meissner, C.A. and Fraser, J. (2010a). The Cognitive Interview: a meta-analytic review and study space analysis of the past 25 years. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 16, 340-372.

Memon, A. and Wright, D.B. (1999). Eyewitness testimony and the Oklahoma bombing. The Psychologist, 12, 292-295.

Memon, A., Zaragoza, M., Clifford, B.R. and Kidd, L. (2010b). Inoculation or antidote? The effects of cognitive interview timing on false memory for forcibly fabricated events. Law and Human Behaviour, 34, 105-117.

Menet, J.S. and Rosbash, M. (2011). When brain clocks lose track of time: cause or consequence of neuropsychiatric disorders. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 21(6), 849-857.

Mershon, D.H., Ballenger, W.L., Little, A.D., McMurtry, R.L. and Buchanan, J.L. (1989). Effects of room reflectance and background noise on perceived auditory distance. Perception, 18, 403-416.

Mershon, D.H. and King, L.E. (1975). Intensity and reverberation as factors in the auditory perception of egocentric distance. Perception and Psychophysics, 18, 409-415.

Mesagno, C., Harvey, J.T. and Janelle, C.M. (2011). Self-presentation origins of choking: evidence from separate pressure manipulations. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 33, 441-459.

Mesagno, C. and Hill, D.M. (2013). Definition of choking in sport: re-conceptualisation and debate. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 44, 267-277.

Mesagno, C. and Marchant, D. (2013). Charac teristics of polar opposites: an exploratory investi gation of choking-resistant and choking-susceptible athletes. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 25, 72-91.

Mickes, L., Flowe, H.D. and Wixted, J.T. (2012). Receiver operating characteristic analysis of eyewitness memory: comparing the diagnostic accuracy of simultaneous versus sequential lineups. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18(4), 361-376.

Miller, G. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Doubleday.

Miller, L.S. and Miller, S.E. (1996). Caffeine enhances initial but not extended learning of a proprioceptive-based discrimination task in nonsmoking moderate users. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 891-898.

Mills, A.W. (1963). Auditory perception and spatial relations. Proceedings of the International Congress on Technology and Blindness, 2, 111-139.

Millsaps, C.L., Azrin, R.L. and Mittenberg, W. (1994). Neuropsychological effects of chronic cannabis use on the memory and intelligence of adolescents. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse, 3(1), 47-55.

Milne, R. (2004). The cognitive interview: A step-by-step guide. Unpublished training manual.

Milne, R. and Bull, R. (1999). Investigative interviewing: Psychology and practice. Chichester: Wiley.

Milne, R. and Bull, R. (2006). Interviewing victims of crime, including children and people with intellectual difficulties. In M.R. Kebbell and G.M. Davies (eds), Practical psychology for forensic investigations (pp. 7-23). Chichester: Wiley.

Milne, R., Clare, I.C.H. and Bull, R. (1999). Interviewing adults with learning disability with the cognitive interview. Psychology, Crime & Law, 5, 81-100.

Milne, R., Clare, I.C.H. and Bull, R. (2002). Interrogative suggestibility among witnesses with mild intellectual disabilities: the use of an adaptation of the GSS. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 15, 1-10.

Milne, R., Poyser, S., Williamson, T. and Savage, S. (2010). Miscarriages of justice: what can we learn? In J. Adler and J. Gray. (eds), Forensic psychology: Concepts, debates and practice (pp. 17-37). Cullompton: Willan.

Milne, R., Shaw, G. and Bull, R. (2007). Investigative interviewing: the role of psychology. In D. Carson, R. Milne, F. Pakes and K. Shalev (eds), Applying psychology to criminal justice (pp. 64-80). Chichester: Wiley.

Milner, A.D. and Goodale, M.A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Minzenberg, M.J. and Carter, C.S. (2008). Modafinil: a review of neurochemical actions and effects on cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(7), 1477-1502.

Mitchell, K.J., Livosky, M. and Mather, M. (1998). The weapon focus effect revisited: the role of novelty. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 287-304.

Miyake, A., Friedman, N.P., Emerson, M.J., Witzki, A.H., Howerter, A. and Wager, T.D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49-100.

Mogford, R.H. (1997). Mental models and situation awareness in air traffic control. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 7, 331-341.

Molnár, M., Boha, R., Czigler, B. and Gaál, Z.A. (2010). The acute effect of alcohol on various memory processes. Journal of Psychophysiology, 24(4), 249-252

Moloney, S. (2010). How menstrual shame affects birth. Women and Birth, 23(4), 153-159.

Monk, T.H. (2005). The post-lunch dip in performance. Clinics in Sports Medicine, 24(2), e15-e23.

Monk, T.H., Weitzman, E.D., Fookson, J.E., Moline, M.L., Kronauer, R.E. and Gander, P.H. (1983). Task variables determine which biological clock controls circadian rhythms in human performance. Nature, 304, 543-545.

Montange, M.F., Van Cauter, E., Refetoff, S., Désir, D., Tourniaire, J. and Copinschi, G. (1981). Effects of ‘jet lag’ on hormonal patterns. II. Adaptation of melatonin circadian periodicity. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 52(4), 642-649.

Moog, H. (1976). The development of musical experience in children of preschool age. Psychol ogy of Music, 4, 38-45.

Moore, L.J., Wilson, M.R., Vine, S.J., Coussens, A.H. and Freeman, P. (2013). Champ or chump? Challenge and threat states during pressurised competition. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 35, 551-562.

Moos, R. (1968). The development of a menstrual distress questionnaire. Psychosomatic Medicine, 30, 853-860.

Mordecai, K.L., Rubin, L.H. and Maki, P.M. (2008). Effects of menstrual cycle phase and oral contraceptive use on verbal memory. Hormones and Behavior, 54(2), 286-293. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.03.006

Morgan, C.A., Hazlett, G., Baranoski, M., Doran, A., Southwick, S. and Loftus, E. (2007). Accuracy of eyewitness identification is significantly associated with performance on a standardized test of face recognition. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 30(3), 213-223.

Morgan, C.A. and Southwick, S. (2014). Perspective: I believe what I remember, but it may not be true. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 112, 101-103.

Morgan, C.A., Southwick, S., Steffian, G., Hazlett, G.A. and Loftus, E.F. (2013). Misinformation can influence memory for recently experienced, highly stressful events. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 36(1), 11-17.

Morgan, M.J. (1998). Recreational use of ‘ecstasy’ (MDMA) is associated with elevated impulsivity. Neuropsychopharmacology, 19, 252-264.

Morgan, M.J. (1999). Memory deficits associated with recreational use of ‘ecstasy’ (MDMA). Psycho pharmacology, 152, 230-248.

Morgan, M.J., McFie, L., Fleetwood, L.H. and Robinson, J.A. (2002). ‘Ecstasy’ (MDMA): are the psychological changes associated with its use reversed by prolonged abstinence. Psycho pharmacology, 159, 294-303.

Mori, R.D., Lam, L. and Gilloux, M. (1987). Learning and plan refinement in a knowledge-based system for automatic speech recognition. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis Machine Intelligence, 9, 289-303.

Moriya, H. and Nittono, H. (2010). Influence of Mood State on the Spatial Gradient of Visual Attention. Paper presented at the Third International Workshop on Kansei, Fukuoka, Japan.

Morris, R.B. and Walter, L.W. (1991). Subtypes of arithmetic-disabled adults: validating childhood findings. In B.P. Rourke (ed), Neuropsychological validation of learning disability subtypes. New York: Guilford Press.

Morrison, C.M. and Conway, M.A. (2010). First words and first memories. Cognition, 21, 1-11.

Morrow, B.A., Elsworth, J.D. and Roth, R.H. (2002). Prenatal cocaine exposure disrupts non-spatial, short-term memory in adolescent and adult male rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 129(1-2), 217-223.

Morrow, D.M., Lee, A. and Rodvold, M. (1993). Analysis of problems in routine controller-pilot communication. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 3, 285-302.

Moser, J.S., Moran, T.P., Schroder, H.S., Donnellan, M.B. and Young, N. (2013). On the relationship between anxiety and error monitoring: a meta-analysis and conceptual framework. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7 (Article 466).

Mosing, M.A., Madison, G., Pedersen, N.L., Kuja-Halkola, R. and Ullén, F. (2014). Practice does not make perfect: no causal effect of music practice on music ability. Psychological Science, 25, 1795-1803.

Most, S.B. and Astur, R.S. (2007). Feature-based attentional set as a cause of traffic accidents. Visual Cognition, 15, 125-132.

Müllensiefen, D. and Wiggins, G. (2011). Sloboda & Parker’s recall paradigm for melodic memory: a new computational perspective. In I. Deliège and J. Davidson (eds), Music and the mind: Essays in honour of John Sloboda (pp. 161-188). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Munsterberg, H. (1908). On the witness stand. New York: Clark Boardman.

Münte, T.F., Nager, W., Beiss, T., Schroeder, C. and Altenmüller, E. (2006). Specialization of the Specialized: electrophysiological Investigations in Professional Musicians. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 131-139.

Mussweiler, T. and Strack, F. (1999). Comparing is believing: a selective accessibility model of judgmental anchoring. European Review of Social Psychology, 10, 135-167.

Mussweiler, T., Strack, F. and Pfeiffer, T. (2000). Overcoming the inevitable anchoring effect: considering the opposite compensates for selective accessibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1142-1150.

Nagarajan, R., Sazali, Y. and Sainarayanan, G. (2004). Computer aided vision assistance for human blind. Integrated Computer-Aided Engineering, 11(1), 15-24.

Nairne, J.S. (2015). Encoding and retrieval: beyond Tulving and Thomson’s (1973) encoding specificity principle. In M.W. Eysenck and D. Groome (eds), Cognitive psychology: Revisiting the classic studies. London: Sage.

National Academy of Science (2014). Identifying the culprit: Assessing eyewitness identification. National Academies Press. Washington, DC.

National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. (2013). Distracted driving 2011. U.S. Department of Transport.

Neil-Sztramko, S.E., Pahwa, M., Demers, P.A. and Gotay, C.C. (2014). Health-related interventions among night shift workers: a critical review of the literature. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environmental & Health, 40(6), 543-556.

Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Freeman.

Neisser, U. (1982). Memorists. In U. Neisser (ed), Memory observed: Remembering in natural contexts. San Francisco: Freeman.

Neisser, U. (1996). Remembering as doing. Behavioural & Brain Sciences, 19, 203-204.

Neisser, U. and Becklen, R. (1975). Selective looking: attending to visually specified events. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 480-494.

Neisser, U. and Harsch, N. (1992). Phantom flashbulbs: false recollections of hearing the news about Challenger. In E. Winograd and U. Neisser (eds), Affect and accuracy in recall: Studies of ‘flashbulb’ memories. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Nelson, K. (1988). The ontogeny of memory for real events. In U. Neisser and E. Winograd (eds), Remembering reconsidered: Ecological and traditional approaches to the study of memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Nelson, K. and Ross, G. (1980). The generalities and specifics of long-term memory in infants and young children. In M. Perlmutter (ed), Children’s memory: New directions for child development. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

Neuhoff, J. (2011). Perception, cognition and auditory displays. In T. Hermann, A. Hunt and J. Neuhoff (eds), The sonification handbook. Berlin: Logos.

Neuhoff, J.G., Kramer, G. and Wayand, J. (2002). Pitch and loudness interact in auditory displays: can the data get lost in the map? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(1), 17-25.

Newcombe, N.S., Drummey, A.B., Fox. N.A., Lie, E. and Ottinger-Alberts, W. (2000). Remembering early childhood: how much, how, and why (or why not). Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 55-58.

Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Newell, B.R. (2005). Re-visions of rationality? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 11-15.

Newell, B.R. (2011). Recognising the recognition heuristic for what is (and what it’s not). Judgment and Decision Making, 6, 409-412.

Newell, B.R. (2015). Wait! Just let me NOT think about that for a minute: what role do implicit processes play in higher level cognition? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 65-70

Newell, B.R. (in press) Decision making under risk: beyond Kahneman and Tversky’s prospect theory. In M. Eysenck and D. Groome (eds), Cognitive psychology: Revisiting the classic studies (pp. 162-178). London: Sage.

Newell, B.R., Lagnado, D.A. and Shanks, D.R. (2015). Straight choices: The psychology of decision making (2nd edn). Hove: Psychology Press.

Newell, B.R., McDonald, R.I., Brewer, M. and Hayes, B.K. (2014). The psychology of environmental decisions. Annual Review of Environment and Resources, 39, 443-467

Newell, B.R. and Pitman, A.J. (2010). The psychology of global warming: improving the fit between the science and the message. Bulletin of the American Meteorological Society, 91, 1003-1014.

Newell, B.R. and Shanks, D.R. (2014a). Unconscious influences on decision making: a critical review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 1-63.

Newell, B.R. and Shanks, D.R. (2014b). Prime numbers: anchoring and its implications for theories of behavior priming. Social Cognition, 32, 88-108.

Newell, B.R., Wong, K.Y., Cheung, J.C. and Rakow, T. (2009). Think, blink or sleep on it? The impact of modes of thought on complex decision making. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 707-32.

Newhouse, P.A., Potter, A. and Singh, A. (2004). Effects of nicotinic stimulation on cognitive performance. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 4(1), 36-46.

Nguyen, N.D., Tucker, M.A., Stickgold, R. and Wamsley, E.J. (2013). Overnight sleep enhances hippocampus-dependent aspects of spatial memory. Sleep, 36(7), 1051-1057.

Nicholls, A., Holt, N., Polman, R. and James, D. (2005). Stress and coping among international adolescent golfers. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 17, 333-340.

Nichols, J.M. and Martin, F. (1998). Social drinking, memory and information processing. In J. Snel and M.M. Lorist (eds), Nicotine, caffeine and social drinking: Behaviour and brain function. Amsterdam: Harwood Academic.

Nicolson, P. (1992). Menstrual cycle research and the construction of female psychology. In J.T.E. Richardson (ed), Cognition and the menstrual cycle. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Nielsen, S.E., Ahmed, I. and Cahill, L. (2013). Sex and menstrual cycle phase at encoding influence emotional memory for gist and detail. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 106, 56-65. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2013.07.015

Niessen, C., Eyferth, K. and Bierwagen, T. (1999). Modelling cognitive processes of experienced air traffic controllers. Ergonomics, 42, 1507-1520.

Niessen, C., Leuchter, S. and Eyferth, K. (1998). A psychological model of air traffic control and its implementation. In F.E. Riter and R.M. Young (eds), Proceedings of the Second European Conference on Cognitive Modelling. Nottingham: Nottingham University Press.

Nietzsche, F., Kaufmann, W. and Hollingdale, R.J. (2015), translated from text first published in 1888. Twilight of the idols: How to Philosophize with the hammer, CreateSpace Independent Publishing Platform (1 February 2015).

Nieuwenhuys, A. and Oudejans, R.R.R. (2012). Anxiety and perceptual-motor performance: to ward an integrated model of concepts, mechanisms, and processes. Psychological Research, 76, 747-759.

Niu, S.F., Chung, M.H., Chen, C.H., Hegney, D., O’Brien, A. and Chou, K.R. (2011). The effect of shift rotation on employee cortisol profile, sleep quality, fatigue, and attention level: a systematic review. Journal of Nursing Research, 19(1), 68-81.

Noon, E. and Hollin, C.R. (1987). Lay knowledge of eyewitness behaviour: a British survey. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 1(2), 143-153.

Norell, K., Läthén, K.B., Bergström, P., Rice, A., Natu, V. and O’Toole, A. (2015). The effect of image quality and forensic expertise in facial image comparisons. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 60(2), 331-340.

Norman, D.A. (1988). The psychology of everyday things. New York: Basic Books.

North, A.C., Hargreaves, D.J. and Hargreaves, J.J. (2004). Uses of music in everyday life. Music Perception, 22(1), 41-77.

Northcraft, G.B. and Neale, M.A. (1987). Experts, amateurs, and real estate: an anchoring-and-adjustment perspective on property pricing decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 39, 84-97.

Nyberg, G. (2014). Developing a ‘somatic velocimeter’: the practical knowledge of free skiers. Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/2159676X.2014.888585

Nygaard, L.C. and Pisoni, D.B. (1998). Talker specific learning in speech perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 60, 355-376.

O’Brien, D. (1993). How to develop a perfect memory. London: Pavilion.

O’Neill, M. and Milne, R. (2014). Success within criminal investigations: is communication a key component? In R. Bull (ed), Investigative interviewing (pp. 123-146). New York: Springer.

Oatley, K. (1974). Circadian rhythms and representations of the environment in motivational systems. In D.G. McFarland (ed), Motivational control systems analysis. London: Academic Press.

Oberauer, K., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O. and Süss, H.M. (2005). Working memory and intelli gence - their correlation and their relation: comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 61-65; author reply 72-75.

Official Charts Company (December, 2014). The Official Top 40 Biggest Selling Singles of 2014. www.officialcharts.com/chart-news/the-official-top-100-biggest-songs-of-2014-revealed__7577/.

Olsen, R.A. (2005). Pedestrian injury issues in litigation. In Y.I. Noy and W. Karwowski (eds), Handbook of human factors litigation. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Olson, P.L., Aoki, T., Battle, D. and Flannagan, M. (1990). Development of a headlight system per formance evaluation tool. Final report: The University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute.

Olson, P.L., Battle, D.S. and Aoki, T. (1989). Driver eye fixations under different operating conditions. The University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute.

Olsson, N., Juslin, P. and Winman, A. (1998). Realism of confidence in earwitness versus eyewitness identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4, 101-118.

ONS. (2015). Adult drinking habits in Great Britain, 2013. Statistical Bulletin. Available from www.ons.gov.uk/ons/dcp171778_395191.pdf

Orasanu, J. and Connolly, T. (1993). The reinvention of decision making. In G.A. Klein, J. Orasanu, R. Calderwood and C.E. Zsambok (eds), Decision making in action: Models and methods (pp. 3-20). Norwood, CT: Ablex.

Orchard, T.L. and Yarmey, A.D. (1995). The effects of whispers, voice-sample duration, and voice distinctiveness on criminal speaker identification. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 249-260.

Ost, J., Granhag, P., Udell, J. and Hjelmsater, E.R. (2008). Familiarity breeds distortion: the effects of media exposure on false reports concerning media coverage of the terrorist attacks in London on 7 July 2005. Memory, 16, 76-85.

Otten, M. (2009). Choking vs. clutch performance under pressure. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 31, 583-601.

Oudejans, R.R.D., Kuijpers, W., Kooijman, C.C. and Bakker, F.C. (2011). Thoughts and attention of athletes under pressure: skill-focus or performance worries? Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 24, 59-73.

Owens, D.A. and Sivak, M. (1996). Differentiation of visibility and alcohol as contributors to twilight road fatalities. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 38(4), 680-689.

Paivio, A. (1965). Abstractness, imagery, and meaningfulness in paired-associate learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 4, 32-38.

Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45, 255-287.

Panksepp, J. and Bernatzky, G. (2002). Emotional sounds and the brain: the neuro-affective foundations of musical appreciation. Behavioural Processes, 80(2), 133-155.

Pansky, A., Koriat, A. and Goldsmith, M. (2005). Eyewitness recall and testimony. In N. Brewer and K.D. Williams (eds), Psychology and law an empirical perspective (pp. 93-150). London: Guilford Press.

Panza, F., Frisardi, V., Seripa, D., Logroscino, G., Santamanto, A., Imbimbo, B.P., Scafato, E., Pilotto, A. and Solfrizzi, P. (2012). Alcohol consumption in mild cognitive impairment and demen tia: harmful or neuroprotective? International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27, 1218-1238.

Papesh, M.H. and Goldinger, S.D. (2014). Infrequent identity mismatches are frequently undetected. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(5), 1335-1349.

Parkin, A.J. and Hunkin, N.M. (2001). British memory research: a journey through the 20th century. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 37-52.

Parlee, M.B. (1974). Stereotypical beliefs about menstruation: a methodological note on the Moos MDQ and some new data. Psychosomatic Medicine 36, 229-240.

Parlee, M.B. (1983). Menstrual rhythms in sensory processes: a review of fluctuations in vision, olfaction, audition, taste and touch. Psychological Bulletin, 93(3), 539-548.

Parrott, A.C. (2001). Human psychopharmacology of Ecstasy (MDMA): a review of 15 years of empirical research. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 16, 557-577.

Parrott, A.C. (2013). MDMA, serotonergic neurotoxicity, and the diverse functional deficits of recreational ‘Ecstasy’ users. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(8), 1466-1484.

Parrott, A.C. and Craig, D. (1992). Cigarette smoking and nicotine gum (0, 2 and 4 mg): effects upon four visual attention tasks. Neuropsychobiology, 25, 34-43.

Parrott, A.C. and Lasky, J. (1998). Ecstasy (MDMA) effects on mood and cognition: before, during and after a Saturday night dance. Psychopharmacology, 139, 261-268.

Pashler, H., Bain, P.M., Bottge, B.A., Graesser, A., McDaniel, M.A. and Metcalfe, J. (2007). Organizing instruction and study to improve student learning (NCER Publication No. 2007-2004). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences, US Department of Education.

Patel, A.D. (2010). Music, language and the brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Patterson, R.D. (1990). Auditory warning sounds in the work environment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 327, 485-492.

Peace, K. and Porter, S. (2004). A longitudinal investigation of the reliability of memories for trauma and other emotional experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1143-1159.

Peretz, I., Vuvan, D., Lagrois, M.É. and Armony, J.L. (2015). Neural overlap in processing music and speech. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 370(1664), 20140090.

Perfect, T.J., Wagstaff, G.F., Moore, D., Andrews, B., Cleveland, V., Newcombe, S., Brisbane, K. and Brown, L. (2008). How can we help witnesses to remember more? It’s an (eyes) open and shut case. Law and Human Behavior, 32, 314-324.

Perreau-Lenz, S., Kalsbeek, A., Garidou, M.L., Wortel, J., Van Der Vliet, J., Van Heijningen, C., Simonneaux, V., Pevet, P. and Buijs, R.M. (2003). Suprachiasmatic control of melatonin synthesis in rats: inhibitory and stimulatory mechanisms. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17(2), 221-228.

Pescod, L. Wilcock, R. and Milne, R. (2013). Improving eyewitness memory in police call centre interviews. Policing: A Journal of Policy and Practice, 7, 299-306.

Petros, T.V., Beckwith, B.E. and Anderson, M. (1990). Individual differences in the effects of time of day and passage difficulty on prose memory in adults. British Journal of Psychology, 81(1), 63-72.

Phillips, P.J. and O’Toole, A.J. (2014). Comparison of human and computer performance across face recognition experiments. Image and Vision Computing, 32, 74-85.

Pihl, R.O., Assaad, J-M. and Bruce, K.R. (1998). Cognition in social drinkers: the interaction of alcohol with nicotine and caffeine. In J. Snel and M.M. Lorist (eds), Nicotine, caffeine and social drinking: Behaviour and brain function. Amsterdam: Harwood Academic.

Pike, P.D. and Carter, R. (2010). Employing cognitive chunking techniques to enhance sight-reading performance of undergraduate group-piano students. International Journal of Music Education, 28(3), 231-246.

Pilcher, J.J., Lambert, B.J. and Huffcutt, A.I. (2000). Differential effects of permanent and rotating shifts on self-report sleep length: a meta-analytic review. Sleep, 23(2), 155-163.

Pillemer, D.B., Goldsmith, L.R., Panter, A.T. and White, S.H. (1988). Very long-term memories of the first year in college. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 709-715.

Pillemer, D.B. and White, S.H. (1989). Childhood events recalled by children and adults. In H.W. Reese (ed), Advances in Child Development and Behaviour. San Diego: Academic Press.

Pisoni, D.B. and Luce, P.A. (1986). Speech perception: research, theory and the principle issues. In E.C. Schwab and H.C. Nusbaum (eds), Pattern recognition by humans and machines, Vol. 1: Speech Perception. London: Academic Press.

Plihal, W. and Born, J. (1997). Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(4), 534-547.

Ponzo, M. (1910). Intorno ad alcune illusioni nel campo delle sensazioni tattili, sull’illusione di Aristotele e fenomeni analoghi (On some tactile illusions, Aristotle’s illusion, and similar phenomena). Archive für die Gesamte Psychologie, 16, 307-345.

Poole, D.A. and Lindsay, D.S. (2001). Children’s eyewitness reports after exposure to misinformation from parents. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 27-50.

Pope, D. and Schweitzer, M. (2011). Is Tiger Woods loss averse? Persistent bias in the face of experience, competition and high stakes. American Economic Review, 101, 129-157.

Pope, H.G., Gruber, A.J., Hudson, J.I., Huestis, M.A. and Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2001). Neuropsychological performance in long-term cannabis users. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(10), 909-915.

Pope, H.G., Gruber, A.J. and Yurgelun-Todd, D. (1995). The residual effects of cannabis: the current status of research. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 38(1), 25-34.

Potts, R. and Shanks, D.R. (2012). Can testing immunize memories against interference? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(6), 1780-1785.

Potts, R. and Shanks, D.R. (2014). The benefit of generating errors during learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2), 644-667.

Poyser, S. and Milne, R. (2011). Miscarriages of justice: a call for continued research focusing on reforming the investigative process. British Journal of Forensic Practice, 13, 61-71.

Pritchard, W.S. and Robinson, J.H. (1998). Effects of nicotine on human performance. In J. Snel and M.M. Lorist (eds), Nicotine, caffeine and social drinking: Behaviour and brain function. Amsterdam: Harwood Academic Publishers.

Provost, S.C. and Woodward, R. (1991). Effects of nicotine gum on repeated administration of the Stroop test. Psychopharmacology, 104, 536-540.

Pruitt, J.S., Cannon-Bowers, J.A. and Salas, E. (1997). In search of naturalistic decisions. In R. Flin, E. Salas, M. Strub and L. Martin (eds), Decision making under stress: Emerging themes and applications (pp. 29-42). Aldershot: Ashgate.

Puts, D.A., Cárdenas, R.A., Bailey, D.H., Burriss, R.P., Jordan, C.L. and Breedlove, S.M. (2010). Salivary testosterone does not predict mental rotation performance in men or women. Hormones and Behavior, 58(2), 282-289. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.03.005

Raaijmakers, J.G.W. and Jacab, E. (2013). Rethinking inhibition theory: on the problematic status of the inhibition theory for forgetting. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 98-122.

Rajaratnam, S.M. and Arendt, J. (2001). Health in a 24-h society. The Lancet, 358(9286), 999-1005.

Rakow, T. and Newell, B.R. (2010). Degrees of uncertainty: an overview and framework for future research on experience-based choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 23, 1-14.

Raugh, M.R. and Atkinson, R.C. (1975). A mnemonic method for learning a second language vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 1-16.

Raz, A., Packard, M.G., Alexander, G.M., Gerianne, M., Buhle, J.T., Zhu, H.T. et al. (2009). A slice of pi: an exploratory neuroimaging study of digit encoding and retrieval in a superior memorist. Neurocase, 15, 361-372.

Reder, L.M. (1987). Strategy selection in question answering. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 90-134.

Reder, L.M. and Anderson, J.R. (1982). Effects of spacing and embellishment for the main points of a text. Memory & Cognition, 10, 97-102.

Redgrove, J. (1971). Menstrual cycles. In W. Colquhoun (ed), Biological rhythms and human performance. New York: Academic Press.

Redick, T.S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T.L., Hicks, K.L., Fried, D.E., Hambrick, D.Z., Kane, M.J. and Engle, R.W. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 359-379.

Repantis, D., Schlattmann, P., Laisney, O. and Heuser, I. (2010). Modafinil and methylphenidate for neuroenhancement in healthy individuals: a systematic review. Pharmacological Research, 62(3), 187-206.

Repp, B.H. (1987). The sound of two hands clapping: an exploratory study. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 81, 1100-1109.

Richards, A. and French, C.C. (1992). An anxiety-related bias in semantic activation when processing threat/neutral homographs, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 503-525.

Richardson, J.T.E. (1989). Student learning and the menstrual cycle: premenstrual symptoms and approaches to studying. Educational Psychology, 9, 215-238.

Richardson, J.T.E. (1992). Memory and the menstrual cycle. In J.T.E. Richardson (ed), Cognition and the menstrual cycle. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Richmond, J. and Nelson, C.A. (2007). Accounting for change in declarative memory: a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Developmental Review, 27, 349-373.

Richmond, L.L., Morrison, A.B., Chein, J.M. and Olson, I.R. (2011). Working memory training and transfer in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 26, 813-822.

Ridley, A.M, Gabbert, F. and La Rooy, D.J. (eds) (2013). Suggestibility in legal contexts: Psychological research and forensic implications. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell.

Riley, J.L., Robinson, M.E., Wise, E.A. and Price, D.D. (1999). A meta-analytic review of pain perception across the menstrual cycle. Pain, 81(3), 225-235.

Rimmele, U., Davachi, L. and Phelps, E.A. (2012). Memory for time and place contributes to enhanced confidence in memories for emotional events. Emotion, 12, 834-846.

Ritchie, K., Carrière, I., de Mendonça, A., Portet, F., Dartigues, J.F., Rouaud, O., Barberger-Gateau, P. and Ancelin, M.L. (2007). The neuroprotective effects of caffeine: a prospective population study (the Three City Study). Neurology, 69, 536-545.

Ritchie, K.L., Smith, F.G., Jenkins, R., Bindemann, M., White, D. and Burton, A.M. (2015). Viewers base estimates of face matching accuracy on their own familiarity: explaining the photo-ID paradox. Cognition, 141, 161-169.

Robazza, C., Bartoli, L. and Nougier, V. (1998). Physiological arousal and performance in elite archers: a field study. European Psychologist, 3, 263-270.

Robert, G. and Hockey, J. (1997). Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload: a cognitive-energetical framework. Biological Psychology, 45(1), 73-93.

Robertson, D. J., Kramer, R. S. and Burton, A. M. (2015). Face Averages Enhance User Recognition for Smartphone Security. PloS one, 10(3), e0119460.

Robinson, M.D. and Tamir, M. (2005). Neuroticism as mental noise: a relation between neuroticism and reaction time standard deviations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 107-114.

Rock, I. (1977). In defense of unconscious inference. In W. Epstein (ed), Stability and constancy in visual perception: Mechanisms and processes (pp. 321-377). New York: Wiley.

Rock, I. (1983). The logic of perception. MA: MIT Press.

Rock, P.B. and Harris, M.G. (2006). τ as a potential control variable for visually guided braking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(2), 251.

Rodgers, J., Buchanan, T., Scholey, A.B., Heffernan, T.M., Ling, J. and Parrott, A. (2001). Differential effects of ecstasy and cannabis on self-reports of memory ability: a web-based study. Human Psycho pharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 16(8), 619-625.

Roebuck, R. and Wilding, J. (1993). Effects of vowel variety and sample length on identification of a speaker in a line up. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 7, 475-481.

Roediger, H.L., III (2014). Make it stick: how memory athletes perform and how their techniques can help you. APS 26th Annual Convention (2014), Cognitive Psychology, Learning, Memory, San Francisco.

Rogers, P.J., Heatherley, S.V., Mullings, E.L. and Smith, J.E. (2013). Faster but not smarter: effects of caffeine and caffeine withdrawal on alertness and performance. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 226(2), 229-240.

Rosekind, M.R., Gregory, K.B., Mallis, M.M., Brandt, S.L., Seal, B. and Lerner, D. (2010). The cost of poor sleep: workplace productivity loss and associated costs. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 52(1), 91-98.

Rowe, G., Hirsh, J.B. and Anderson, A.K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(1), 383-388.

Rubin, D.C., Rahal, T.A. and Poon, L.W. (1998). Things learned in early childhood are remembered best. Memory & Cognition, 26, 3-19.

Rubin, D.C., Wetzler, S.E. and Nebes, R.D. (1986). Autobiographical memory across the lifespan. In D.C. Rubin (ed), Autobiographical memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ruble, D.N. (1977). Premenstrual symptoms: a reinterpretation. Science, 197, 291-292.

Ruby, N.F., Hwang, C.E., Wessells, C., Fernandez, F., Zhang, P., Sapolsky, R. and Heller, H.C. (2008). Hippocampal-dependent learning requires a functional circadian system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 15593-15598.

Rumar, K. (1985). The role of perceptual and cognitive filters in observed behavior. In L. Evans and R. Schwing (eds), Human behavior in traffic safety. New York: Plenum Press.

Russell, R., Duchaine, B. and Nakayama, K. (2009). Super-recognisers: people with extraordinary face recognition ability. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 252-257.

Ryback, R.S. (1971). The continuum and specificity of the effects of alcohol on memory. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcoholism, 32, 215-216.

Sabey, B. and Staughton, G.C. (1975). Interacting roles of road environment, vehicle and road user. Paper presented at the 5th International Con ference of the International Association for Accident Traffic Medicine, London.

Sack, R.L., Blood, M.L. and Lewy, A.J. (1992). Melatonin rhythms in night shift workers. Sleep, 15(5), 434-441.

Sale, M.V., Ridding, M.C. and Nordstrom, M.A. (2007). Factors influencing the magnitude and reproducibility of corticomotor excitability changes induced by paired associative stimulation. Experimental Brain Research, 181(4), 615-626.

Sale, M.V., Ridding, M.C. and Nordstrom, M.A. (2008). Cortisol inhibits neuroplasticity induction in human motor cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(33), 8285-8293.

Salthouse, T.A. (2015). Individual differences in working memory and aging. In R.H. Logie and R.G. Morris (eds), Working memory and ageing (pp. 1-20). Hove: Psychology Press.

Salthouse, T.A. and Babcock, R.L. (1991). Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Developmental Psychology, 27, 763-776.

Saltz, E. (1970). Manifest anxiety: have we misread the data? Psychological Review, 77, 568-573.

Sammler, D., Koelsch, S., Ball, T., Brandt, A., Elger, C.E., Friederici, A.D., Grigutsch, M., Huppertz, H.-J., Knösche, T.R., Wellmer, J., Widman, G. and Schulze-Bonhage, A. (2009). Overlap of Musical and linguistic syntax processing: intracranial ERP Evidence. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 494-498.

Samson, S. and Peretz, I. (2005). Effects of prior exposure on music liking and recognition in patients with temporal lobe lesions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060(1), 419-428.

Sanders, G.S. (1986). The usefulness of eyewitness research from the perspective of police investigators. Unpublished manuscript, State University of New York. Cited in R. Fisher, R.E. Geiselman and M. Armador. (1989). Field test of the cognitive interview: enhancing the recollection of actual victims and witnesses of crime. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 722-727.

Sanderson, P.M., Liu, D. and Jenkins, S.A. (2009). Auditory displays in anaesthesiology. Current Opinion in Anaesthesiology, 22, 788-795.

Santos, C., Costa, J., Santos, J., Vaz-Carneiro, A. and Lunet, N. (2010). Caffeine intake and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 20(Suppl.1), 187-204.

Santos, V.G.F., Santos, V.R.F., Felippe, L.J.C., Almeida, J.W. Jnr, Bertuzzi, R., Kiss, M.A.P.D and Lima-Silva, A.E. (2014). Caffeine reduces reaction time and improves performance in simulated-contest of taekwondo. Nutrients, 692, 637-649.

Satinder, K.P. and Mastronardi, L.M. (1974). Sex differences in figural aftereffects as a function of phase of the menstrual cycle. Psychologia, 17, 1-5.

Savage, L.J. (1954). The foundations of statistics. New York: Wiley.

Savage, S. and Milne, R. (2007). Miscarriages of justice - the role of the investigative process. In T. Newburn, T. Williamson and A. Wright. (eds), Handbook of criminal investigation (pp. 610-627). Cullompton: Willan.

Savulich, G., Shergill, S. and Yiend, J. (2012). Biased cognition in psychosis. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3, 514-536, doi: 10.5127/jep.016711

Sawyer, R.K. (2005). Music and conversation. Musical Communication, 45-60.

Scaletti, C. and Craig, A.B. (1991). Using sound to extract meaning from complex data. In E.J. Farnell and T.J. Watson (eds), Proceedings of the SPIE Conference 1459, Extracting Meaning from Complex Data: Processing, Display, Interaction II. San Jose, CA: Society of Photo Optical.

Schabus, M., Gruber, G., Parapatics, S., Sauter, C., Klösch, G., Anderer, P., Klimesch, W., Saletu, B. and Zeitlhofer, J. (2004). Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation. Sleep, 27(8), 1479-1485.

Scheer, F.A., Hilton, M.F., Mantzoros, C.S. and Shea, S.A. (2009). Adverse metabolic and cardiovascular consequences of circadian misalignment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(11), 4453-4458.

Scheer, F.A. and Buijs, R.M. (1999). Light affects morning salivary cortisol in humans. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 84(9), 3395-3398.

Scheer, F.A., Shea, T.J., Hilton, M.F. and Shea, S.A. (2008). An endogenous circadian rhythm in sleep inertia results in greatest cognitive impairment upon awakening during the biological night. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 23(4), 353-361.

Schiff, W. and Detwiler, M.L. (1979). Information used in judging impending collision. Perception, 8(6), 647-658.

Schmeck, A., Mayer, R., Opfermann, M., Pfeiffer, V. and Leutner, D. (2014). Drawing pictures during learning from scientific text: testing the generative drawing effect and the prognostic drawing effect. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39(4), 275-286.

Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C. and Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(7), 755-789.

Schmidt, D.M. and Bjork, R.A. (1992). New conceptualisations of practice: common principles in three paradigms suggest new concepts for training. Psychological Science, 3, 207-217.

Schmithorst, V.J. and Brown, R.D. (2004). Empirical validation of the triple-code model of numerical processing for complex math operations using functional MRI and group independent component analysis of the mental addition and subtraction of fractions. NeuroImage, 22, 1419-1425.

Schmolk, H., Buffalo, E.A. and Squire, L.R. (2000). Memory distortions develop over time: recollections of the O.J. Simpson trial verdict after 15 and 32 months. Psychological Science, 11, 39-45.

Schulkind, M.D., Hennis, L.K. and Rubin, D.C. (1999). Music, emotion, and autobiographical memory: they’re playing your song. Memory & Cognition, 27, 948-955.

Schweitzer, P.K., Randazzo, A.C., Stone, K., Erman, M. and Walsh, J.K. (2006). Laboratory and field studies of naps and caffeine as practical countermeasures for sleep-wake problems associated with night work. Sleep, 29(1), 39-50.

Searleman, A. and Herrmann, D. (1994). Memory from a broader perspective. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Sedlmeier, P., Hertwig, R. and Gigerenzer, G. (1998). Are judgments of the positional frequencies of letters systematically biased due to availability? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 754-770.

Segal, Z.V., Williams, J.M.G. and Teasdale, J.D. (2002). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: a new approach to preventing relapse, New York: Guilford Press.

Shah, P. and Miyake, A. (1996). The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: an individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 4-27.

Shah, P. and Miyake, A. (1999). Models of working memory: an introduction. In A. Miyake and P. Shah (eds), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Shanteau, J. (1992). Competence in experts: the role of task characteristics. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 53, 252-266.

Shapley, R. (1995). Parallel neural pathways and visual function. In M.S. Gazzaniga (ed), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 315-324). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Shaw, J. and Porter, S. (2015). Constructing rich false memories of committing crime. Psychological Science, 26(3), 291-301.

Shaw, J.S., Bjork, R.A. and Handal, A. (1995). Retrieval-induced forgetting in an eyewitness-memory paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 249-253.

Shephard, R.N. (1981). Psychophysical complementarity. In M. Kubovy and J.R. Pimerantz (eds), Perceptual organization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Shepherd, E. and Griffiths, A. (2013). Investigative interviewing: The conversation management approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Sheppard, T.L. (2013). Caffeine buzz. Nature Chemical Biology, 9(290), 1202-1204.

Sherwood, N., Kerr, J.S. and Hindmarch, I. (1992). Psychomotor performance in smokers following single and repeated doses of nicotine gum. Psychopharmacology, 108, 432-436.

Shinoda, H., Hayhoe, M.M. and Shrivastava, A. (2001). What controls attention in natural environments? Vision Research, 41(25), 3535-3545.

Shipstead, Z., Redick, T.S. and Engle, R.W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138 (4), 628-654.

Shute, V.J. (1991). Who is likely to acquire programming skills? Journal of Educational Computing Research, 7, 1-24.

Sierra, M. and Berrios, G.E. (2000). Flashbulb and flashback memories. In G.E. Berrios and J.R. Hodges (eds), Memory disorders in psychiatric practice. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Signal, T.L., Van Den Berg, M.J., Mulrine, H.M. and Gander, P.H. (2012). Duration of sleep inertia after napping during simulated night work and in extended operations. Chronobiology International, 29(6), 769-779.

Simcock, G. and Hayne, H. (2003). Age-related changes in verbal and non-verbal memory during early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 39, 805-814.

Simon, D.A. and Bjork, R.A. (2001). Metacognition in motor learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 907-912.

Simon, H.A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of environments. Psychological Review, 63, 129-138.

Simon, H.A. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 1-19.

Simon, H.A. (1992). What is an explanation of behavior? Psychological Science, 3, 150-61.

Simons, D.J. and Chabris, C.F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059-1074.

Sloboda, J.A. (1985). The musical mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

Sloboda, J.A. (2005). Exploring the musical mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

Sloboda, J.A. and Edworthy, J. (1981). Attending to two melodies at once: the effect of key relatedness. Psychology of Music, 9, 39-43.

Sloboda, J.A. and Juslin, P.N. (2010). At the interface between the inner and outer world. In P.N. Juslin and J.A. Sloboda (eds), Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 73-98). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Sloboda, J.A. and Parker, D.H. (1985). Immediate recall of melodies. In P.I.C. Howell and R. West (eds), Musical structure and cognition (pp. 143-167). London: Academic Press.

Smalarz, L. and Wells, G.L. (2015). Contamination of eyewitness self-reports and the mistaken-identification problem. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(2), 120-124.

Smart, R.G., Mann, R.E. and Stoduto, G. (2003). The prevalence of road rage: estimates from Ontario. Canadian Journal of Public Health/Revue Canadienne de Sante’e Publique, 247-250.

Smieszek, H., Manske, P., Hasselberg, A., Russwinkel, N. and Moehlenbrink, C. (2013). Cognitive simulation of limited working memory capacity applied to an air traffic control task. Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Cognitive Modelling. Ottawa: Canada.

Smith, A.P. (1998). Effects of caffeine on attention: low levels of arousal. In J. Snel and M.M. Lorist (eds), Nicotine, caffeine and social drinking: Behaviour and brain function. Amsterdam: Harwood Academic.

Smith, A.P., Rusted, J.M., Eaton-Williams, P. and Hall, S.R. (1991). The effects of caffeine, impulsivity and time of day on performance, mood and cardiovascular function. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 5(2), 120-128.

Smith, A.P., Rusted, J.M., Eaton-Williams, P, Savory, M. and Leathwood, P. (1990). Effects of caffeine given before and after lunch on sustained attention. Neuropsychobiology, 23, 160-163.

Smith, A.P., Sturgess, W. and Gallagher, J. (1999). Effects of a low doses of caffeine given in different drinks on mood and performance. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 14, 473-482.

Smith, C. (2001). Sleep states and memory processes in humans: procedural versus declarative memory systems. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 5(6), 491-506.

Smith, S.M., Glenberg, A.M. and Bjork, R.A. (1978). Environmental context and human memory. Memory & Cognition, 6, 342-353.

Smyth, M.M., Collins, A.F., Morris, P.E. and Levy, P. (1994). Cognition in action (2nd edn). Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Snook, B. and Keating, K. (2010). A field study of adult witness interviewing practices in a Canadian police organization. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 16, 160-172.

Snyder, S.H. (1996). Drugs and the brain. New York: Scientific American Library.

Social Issues Research Centre (1998). Social and cultural aspects of drinking: A report to the European Commission. Oxford: Social Issues Research Centre. Available from www.sirc.org/publik/social_drinking.pdf [accessed 5 June 2015].

Soderstrom, N.C. and Bjork, R.A. (2015). Learning versus performance: an integrative review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 176-199.

Sofuoglu, M. and Sewell, R.A. (2009). Norepinephrine and stimulant addiction. Addiction Biology, 14(2), 119-129.

Solowij, N. (1995). Do cognitive impairments recover following cessation of cannabis use? Life Sciences, 56(23-24), 2119-2126.

Sommer, B. (1992). Cognitive performance and the menstrual cycle. In J.T.E. Richardson (ed), Cognition and the menstrual cycle. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Souza, E.G., Ramos, M.G., Hara, C.P., Stumpf, B. and Rocha, F.L. (2012). Neuropsychological performance and menstrual cycle: a literature review. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 34(1), 5-12.

Spencer, M.B., Robertson, K.A. and Folkard, S. (2006). The development of a fatigue/risk index for shiftworkers. Health and safety executive report no.466.

Spronk, D.B., van Wel, J.H.P., Ramaekers, J.G. and Verkes, R.J. (2013). Characterizing the cognitive effects of cocaine: a comprehensive review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(8), 1838-1859.

Steblay, N. (1992). A meta-analytic review of the weapon focus effect. Law and Human Behavior, 16, 413-424.

Steblay, N.K. (2011). What we know now: the Evanston Illinois field lineups. Law and Human Behavior, 35(1), 1-12.

Steblay, N.K, Dysart, J., Fulero, S. and Lindsay, R.C. (2001). Eyewitness accuracy rates in sequential and simultaneous lineup presentations: a meta-analytic comparison. Law and Human Behavior, 25(5), 459-473.

Steblay, N.K., Dysart, J.E. and Wells, G.L. (2011). Seventy-two tests of the sequential lineup superiority effect: a meta-analysis and policy discussion. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 17(1), 99-139.

Steele, C.M. and Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 797-811. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797

Stein, E.S. and Garland, D. (1993). Air traffic controller working memory: Considerations in air traffic control tactical operations. Technical Report DOT/FAA/CT-TN 93/37. Directorate for Aviation Technology, NJ: Federal Aviation Administration.

Stein, L.M. and Memon, A. (2006). Testing the efficacy of the cognitive interview in a developing country. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 597-605.

Stephan, F.K. and Kovacevic, N.S. (1978). Multiple retention deficit in passive avoidance in rats is eliminated by suprachiasmatic lesions. Behavioral Biology, 22(4), 456-462.

Sterman, J.D. (2011). Communicating climate change risks in a skeptical world. Climatic Change, 108, 811-826.

Stickgold, R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature, 437, 1272-1278.

Storm, B.C. and Levy, B.J. (2012). A progress report on the inhibitory account of retrieval-induced forgetting. Memory & Cognition, 40, 827-843.

Storr, A. (1992). Music and the mind, London: Harper Collins.

Strack, F. and Mussweiler, T. (1997). Explaining the enigmatic anchoring effect: mechanisms of selective accessibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 437-446.

Strayer, D.L., Drews, F.A. and Crouch, D.J. (2006). A comparison of the cell phone driver and the drunk driver. Human Factors, 48(2), 381-391.

Streufert, S. and Pogash, R. (1998). Limited alcohol consumption and complex task performance. In J. Snel and M.M. Lorist (eds), Nicotine, caffeine and social drinking: Behaviour and brain function. Amsterdam: Harwood Academic.

Streufert, S., Pogash, R., Miller, J., Gingrich, D., Landis, R., Lonardi, L., Severs, W. and Roache, J.D. (1995). Effects of caffeine deprivation on complex human functioning. Psychopharmacology, 118, 377-384.

Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643-662.

Sullivan, J.M. and Flannagan, M.J. (2002). The role of ambient light level in fatal crashes: inferences from daylight saving time transitions. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 34(4), 487-498.

Supa, M., Cotzin, M. and Dallenbach, K.M. (1944). Facial vision: the perception of obstacles by the blind. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 133-183.

Susilo, T. and Duchaine, B. (2013). Advances in developmental prosopagnosia research. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(3), 423-429.

Sylvester, C.M., Corbetta, M., Raichle, M.E., Rodebaugh, T.L., Schlagger, B.L., Sheline, Y.I., et al. (2012). Functional network dysfunction in anxiety and anxiety disorders. Topics in Neuroscience, 35, 527-535.

Talarico, J.M. and Rubin, D.C. (2003). Confidence, not consistency, characterises flashbulb memories. Psychological Science, 14, 455-461.

Tanaka, J.W., Heptonstall, B. and Hagen, S. (2013). Perceptual expertise and the plasticity of other-race face recognition. Visual Cognition, 21, 1183-1201.

Tassi, P. and Muzet, A. (2000). Sleep inertia. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 4(4), 341-353.

Teasdale, J.D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and Emotion, 2(3), 247-274.

Tekcan, A.I. and Peynircioglu, Z.F. (2002). Effects of age on flashbulb memories. Psychology and Aging, 17, 416-422.

Tervaniemi, M., Kruck, S., De Baene, W., Schröger, E., Alter, K. and Friederici, A.D. (2009). Top-down modulation of auditory processing: effects of sound context, musical expertise and attentional focus. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(8), 1636-1642.

Testa, M., Fillmore, M.T., Norris, J., Abbey, A., Curtin, J.J., Leonard, K.E., Mariano, K.A. et al. (2006). Understanding alcohol expectancy effects: revisiting the placebo condition. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 30(2), 339-348.

Thaler, R.H. and Sunstein, C.R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

The Economist. (February 7, 2014). The market for paternalism: nudge unit leaves kludge unit. www.economist.com/blogs/freeexchange/2014/02/market-paternalism

Thomas, M.H. and Wang, A.Y. (1996). Learning by the keyword mnemonic: looking for long-term benefits. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2, 330-342.

Thorn, L., Hucklebridge, F., Esgate, A., Evans, P. and Clow, A. (2004). The effect of dawn simulation on the cortisol response to awakening in healthy participants. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 925-930.

Thorndike, E.L. (1914). The psychology of learning. New York: Teachers College.

Tipper, S.P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A(2), 321-343.

Toner, J. and Moran, A. (2014). In praise of conscious awareness: a new framework for the investigation of ‘continuous improvement’ in expert athletes. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 (Article 769).

Touitou, Y., Motohashi, Y., Reinberg, A., Touitou, C., Bourdeleau, P., Bogdan, A. and Auzéby, A. (1990). Effect of shift work on the night-time secretory patterns of melatonin, prolactin, cortisol and testosterone. European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology, 60(4), 288-292.

Trbovich, P.L. and LeFevre, J-A. (2003). Phonological and visual working memory in mental addition. Memory & Cognition, 31, 738-745.

Trehub, S.E., Thorpe, L.A. and Trainor, L.J. (1990). Infants’ perception of good and bad melodies. Psychomusicology, 9, 5-19.

Trick, L.M., Brandigampola, S. and Enns, J.T. (2012). How fleeting emotions affect hazard perception and steering while driving: the impact of image arousal and valence. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 45, 222-229. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2011.07. 006

Tsuruhara, A., Nakato, E., Otsuka, Y., Kanazawa, S., Yamaguchi, M.K. and Hill, H. (2011). The hollow-face illusion in infancy: do infants see a screen based rotating hollow mask as hollow? i-Perception, 2, 418-427.

Tuckey, M.R. and Brewer, N. (2003a). The influence of schemas, stimulus ambiguity, and interview schedule on eyewitness memory over time. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 9, 101-118.

Tuckey, M.R. and Brewer, N. (2003b). How schemas affect eyewitness memory over repeated retrieval attempts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 785-800.

Tulving, E. (1962). Subjective organisation in free recall of ‘unrelated’ words. Psychological Review, 69, 344-354.

Tulving, E. (1976). Ecphoric processes in recall and recognition. In J. Brown (ed), Recall and recognition. New York: Wiley

Tulving, E. and Pearlstone, Z. (1966). Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 5, 381-391.

Tulving, E. and Thomson, D.M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80, 352-373.

Turner, M.L. and Engle, R.W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127-154.

Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207-232.

Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124-1131.

Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90, 293-315.

Tyrrell, R.A., Wood, J.M. and Carberry, T.P. (2004). On-road measures of pedestrians’ estimates of their own nighttime conspicuity. Journal of Safety Research, 35(5), 483-490.

Tyrrell, R.A., Wood, J.M., Chaparro, A., Carberry, T.P., Chu, B.-S. and Marszalek, R.P. (2009). Seeing pedestrians at night: visual clutter does not mask biological motion. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 41(3), 506-512.

UK Music (2013). The Economic Contribution of the Core UK Music Industry (summary report).

Underwood, B.J. (1969). Attributes of memory. Psychological Review, 76, 559-573.

Ungerleider, L.G. and Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual sysems. In D.J. Ingle, M.A. Goodale and R.J.W. Mansfield (eds), Analysis of visual behaviour. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Unsworth, N., Redick, T.S., Spillers, G.J. and Brewer, G.A. (2013). Variation in working memory capacity and cognitive control: goal maintenance and microadjustments of control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 326-355.

USDHHS (1988). The health consequences of smoking: nicotine addiction. A report of the Surgeon General. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office.

Ussher, J. (1989). The psychology of the female body. London: Routledge.

Ussher, J. (1992). The demise of dissent and the rise of cognition in menstrual cycle research. In J.T.E. Richardson (ed), Cognition and the menstrual cycle. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Ussher, J.M. (2006). Managing the monstrous feminine: Regulating the reproductive body. London; Routledge.

Vaez Mousavi, S.M.K., Naji, M., Hassanzadeh, N. and Esmaeilpour, M.H. (2011). Arousal and activation in a pistol shooting task. Journal of Military Medicine, 12, 185-190.

Valdez, P., Ramírez, C., García, A., Talamantes, J., Armijo, P. and Borrani, J. (2005). Circadian rhythms in components of attention. Biological Rhythm Research, 36(1-2), 57-65.

Valdez, P., Reilly, T. and Waterhouse, J. (2008). Rhythms of mental performance. Mind, Brain, and Education, 2(1), 7-16.

Valentine, T. (1988). Upside-down faces: a review of the effect of inversion upon face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 79(4), 471-491.

Valentine, T. and Mesout, J. (2009). Eyewitness identification under stress in the London dungeon. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 151-161.

Vallée-Tourangeau, F., Sirota, M. and Villejoubert, G. (2013). Reducing the impact of math anxiety on mental arithmetic: the importance of distributed cognition. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz and I. Wachsmuth (eds), Proceedings of the Thirty-Fifth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3615-3620). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

Van Amelsvoort, L.G., Jansen, N.W., Swaen, G.M., van den Brandt, P.A. and Kant, I. (2004). Direction of shift rotation among three-shift workers in relation to psychological health and work-family conflict. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment & Health, 30(2), 149-156.

Van Bommel, W.J.M. and Tekelenburg, J. (1986). Visibility research for road lighting based on a dynamic situation. Lighting Research and Technology, 18(1), 37-39.

Van der Stelt, O. and Snel, J. (1998). Caffeine and human performance. In J. Snel and M.M. Lorist (eds), Nicotine, caffeine and social drinking: Behaviour and brain function. Amsterdam: Harwood Academic.

Van Derveer, N.J. (1979). Ecological acoustics: human perception of environmental sounds. Dissertation Abstracts International, 40/09B, 4543 (University Microfilms #8004002).

Van Dongen, H.P. and Dinges, D.F. (2005). Circadian rhythms in sleepiness, alertness, and performance. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, 4, 435-443.

Van Dongen, H.P., Price, N.J., Mullington, J.M., Szuba, M.P., Kapoor, S.C. and Dinges, D.F. (2001). Caffeine eliminates psychomotor vigilance deficits from sleep inertia. Sleep, 24(7), 813.

Van Koppen, P. and Lochun, S. (1997). Portraying perpetrators: the validity of offender descriptions by witnesses. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 661-685.

Van Wallendael, L.R., Surace, A., Parsons, D.H. and Brown, M. (1994). ‘Earwitness’ voice recognition: factors affecting accuracy and impact on jurors. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 661-677.

Veronese, C., Richards, J.B., Pernar, L., Sullivan, A.M. and Schwarzstein, R.M. (2013). A randomized pilot study of the use of concept maps to enhance problem-based learning among first-year medical students. Medical Teacher, 35, E1478-E1484.

Ververidis, D. and Kotropoulos, C. (2006). Emotional speech recognition: resources, features and methods. Speech Communication, 48(9), 1162-1181.

Vickers, J.N. (1996). Visual control when aiming at a far target. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 342-354.

Victor, M. (1992). The effects of alcohol on the nervous system. In J.H. Mendelson and N.K. Mello (eds), Medical diagnosis and treatment of alcoholism. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Viitasalo, K., Puttonen, S., Kuosma, E., Lindström, J. and Härmä, M. (2015). Shift rotation and age - interactions with sleep-wakefulness and inflammation. Ergonomics, 58(1), 65-74.

Vine, S.J., Moore, L.J. and Wilson, M.R. (2011). Quiet eye training facilitates competitive putting performance in elite golfers. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(8), 1-9.

Vlach, H.A., Sandhofer, C.M. and Bjork, R.A. (2014). Equal spacing and expanding schedules in children’s categorization and generalization. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 123, 129-137.

von Neumann, J. and Morgenstern, O. (1947). Theory of games and economic behaviour (2nd edn). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Vonmoos, M., Hulka, L.M., Preller, K.H., Jenni, D., Baungartner, M.R., Stohler, R., Bolla, K.I. and Quewdnow, B.B. (2013). Cognitive dysfunctions in recreational and dependent cocaine users: role of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, craving and early age at onset. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 203(1), 35-43.

Voss, M.W., Kramer, A.F., Basak, C., Prakash, R.S. and Roberts, B. (2010). Are expert athletes ‘expert’ in the cognitive laboratory? A meta-analytic review of cognition and sport expertise. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 812-826.

Vredeveldt, A., Tredoux, C.G., Nortje, A., Kempen, K., Puljevic, C. and Labuschagne, G.N. (in press). A field evaluation of the Eye-Closure Interview with witnesses of serious crimes. Law and Human Behavior.

Vredeveldt, A., Van Koppen, P. and Granhag, P.A. (2014). The inconsistent suspect: a systematic review of consistency in truth tellers and liars. In R.H. Bull (ed), Investigative interviewing (pp. 183-207). New York: Springer.

Vrij, A. (2008). Detecting lies and deceit. Chichester: Wiley.

Vrij, A., Leal, S., Mann, S., Vernham, Z. and Femke, B. (in press). Translating theory into practice: evaluating a cognitive lie detection training workshop. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition.

Vrij, A., Mann, S., Fisher, R., Leal, S., Milne, R. and Bull, R. (2008). Increasing cognitive load to facilitate lie detection. Law and Human Behavior, 28, 253-265.

Vuilleumier, P. and Huang, Y.M. (2009). Emotional attention uncovering the mechanisms of affective biases in perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(3), 148-152.

Wadrop, R. (1995). Simpson’s paradox and the hot hand in basketball. The American Statistician, 49, 24-28.

Wagenaar, W.A. (1986). My memory: a study of autobiographical memory over six years. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 225-252.

Wagenaar, W.A. and Van Der Schrier, J.H. (1996). Face recognition as a function of distance and illumination: a practical tool for use in the courtroom. Psychology, Crime & Law, 2(4), 321-332.

Wagner, U., Fischer, S. and Born, J. (2002). Changes in emotional responses to aversive pictures across periods rich in slow-wave sleep versus rapid eye movement sleep. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64(4), 627-634.

Wagstaff, A.S. and Lie, J.A.S. (2011). Shift and night work and long working hours-a systematic review of safety implications. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment & Health, 173-185.

Waldfogel, S. (1948). The frequency and affective character of childhood memories. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 62 (whole issue).

Walker, A. (1992). Men’s and women’s beliefs about the influence of the menstrual cycle on academic performance: a preliminary study. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22(11), 896-909.

Walker, A.E. (1997). The menstrual cycle. London: Routledge.

Walker, B.N. and Lindsay, J. (2005). Using virtual environments to prototype auditory navigation displays. Assistive Technology, 17, 72-81.

Walsh, D. and Bull, R. (2012). Examining rapport in investigative interviews with suspects: does its building and maintenance work? Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology, 27, 73-84.

Walsh, D. and Bull, R. (in press). The association between interview skills, questioning and evidence disclosure strategies, and interview outcomes. Psychology, Crime & Law, 21, 661-680.

Wang, X.S., Armstrong, M.E.G., Cairns, B.J., Key, T.J. and Travis, R.C. (2011). Shift work and chronic disease: the epidemiological evidence. Occupational Medicine, 61(2), 78-89.

Wang, Y. and Patel, S. (2009). Exploring the cognitive foundations of software engineering. International Journal of Software Science and Computational Intelligence, 1, 1-19.

Warburton, D.M. (1995). The effects of caffeine on cognition with and without caffeine abstinence. Psychopharmacology, 119, 66-70.

Warburton, D.M. and Mancuso, G. (1998). Evaluation of the information processing and mood effects of a transdermal nicotine patch. Psychopharmacology, 135(3), 305-310.

Warburton, D.M. and Wesnes, K. (1984). Drugs as research tools in psychology: cholinergic drugs and information processing. Neuropsychobiology, 11, 121-132.

Wareing, M., Fisk, M. and Murphy, J.E. (2000). Working memory deficits in current and previous users of MDMA (‘ecstasy’). British Journal of Psychology, 91, 181-188.

Warnick, D. and Sanders, G. (1980). Why do witnesses make so many mistakes? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10, 362-367

Warren, R.M. (1970). Perceptual restorations of missing speech sounds. Science, 167, 392-393.

Warren, R.M. (1982). Auditory perception: A new synthesis. New York: Pergamon Press.

Warren, R.M. and Warren, R.P (1970). Auditory illusions and confusions. Scientific American, 223, 30-36.

Warren, W.H. and Verbrugge. R.R. (1984). Auditory perception of breaking and bouncing events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 704-712.

Waterhouse, J., Reilly, T. and Atkinson, G. (1997). Jet-lag. The Lancet, 350(9091), 1611-1616.

Waterhouse, J., Reilly, T., Atkinson, G. and Edwards, B. (2007). Jet lag: trends and coping strategies. The Lancet, 369(9567), 1117-1129.

Wegner, K. and Karron, D.B. (1998). Audio-guided blind biopsy needle placement. Medicine Meets Virtual Reality: Art, Science, Technology: Healthcare Evolution, 50, 90-95.

Weinberg, R.S. (1978). Effects of success and failure on patterning of neuromuscular energy. Journal of Motor Behavior, 10, 53-61.

Weiss, M.W., Trehub, S.E. and Schellenberg, E.G. (2012). Something in the way she sings enhanced memory for vocal melodies. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1074-1078.

Wells, G.L. (1978). Applied eyewitness-testimony research: system variables and estimator variables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(12), 1546-1557.

Wells, G.L. and Loftus, E.L. (2003). Eyewitness memory for people and events. In I.B. Weiner. (eds), Handbook of psychology (2nd edn). Part three. Chichester: Wiley.

Wells, G.L., Malpass, R.S., Lindsay, R.C.L., Fisher, R.P., Turtle, J.W. and Fulero, S.M. (2000). From the lab to the police station. A successful application of eyewitness research. American Psychologist, 55, 581-598.

Wells, G.L., Rydell, S.M. and Seelau, E.P. (1993). The selection of distractors for eyewitness lineups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78(5), 835-844.

Wells, G.L., Small, M., Penrod, S., Malpass, R.S., Fulero, S.M. and Brimacombe, C.E. (1998). Eyewitness identification procedures: recommendations for lineups and photospreads. Law and Human Behavior, 22(6), 603-647.

Wells, G.L., Steblay, N.K. and Dysart, J.E. (2015). Double-blind photo lineups using actual eyewitnesses: an experimental test of a sequential versus simultaneous lineup procedure. Law and Human Behavior, 39(1), 1-14.

Wesnes, K. and Revell, A. (1984). The separate and combined effects of nicotine and scopolamine on human information processing. Psychopharmacology, 84, 5-11.

Wesnes, K. and Warburton, D.M. (1983). Effects of smoking on rapid information processing per formance. Neuropsychobiology, 9(4), 223-229.

Wesnes, K. and Warburton, D.M. (1984). Effects of scopolamine and nicotine on human rapid information processing performance. Psychopharmacology, 82(3), 147-150.

Westera, N., Kebbell, M. and Milne, R. (2011). Interviewing witnesses: do investigative and evidential requirements concur? British Journal of Forensic Practice, 13, 103-113.

Westwood, D.A. and Goodale, M.A. (2011). Converging evidence for diverging pathways: neuropsychology and psychophysics tell the same story. Vision Research, 51(8), 804-811.

White, D., Burton, A.M., Jenkins, R. and Kemp, R.I. (2014b). Redesigning photo-ID to improve unfamiliar face matching performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 20(2), 166-173.

White, D., Kemp, R.I., Jenkins, R. and Burton, A.M. (2014c). Feedback training for facial image comparison. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(1), 100-106.

White, D., Kemp, R.I., Jenkins, R., Matheson, M. and Burton, A.M. (2014a). Passport Officers’ Errors in Face Matching. PLOS ONE, 9(8), e103510.

White, D., Phillips, P.J., Hahn, C.A., Hill, M., & O’Toole, A.J. (2015). Perceptual expertise in forensic facial image comparison. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282, 1814-1822.

White, L.K., Suway, J.G., Pine, D.S., Bar-Haim, Y. and Fox, N.A. (2011). Cascading effects: the influence of attention bias to threat on the interpretation of ambiguous information. Behav iour Research and Therapy, 49, 244-251.

Whitfield, D. and Jackson, A. (1982). The air traffic controller’s picture as an example of a mental model. In G. Johansen and J.E. Rijnsdorp (eds), Proceedings of the IFAC conference on analysis, design and evaluation of man-machine systems. London: Pergamon Press.

Wickelgren, W.A. (1964). Size of rehearsal group and short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 413-419.

Wickens, C.D. (2000). Engineering psychology and human performance (3rd edn). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Wickens, C.D. and Alexander, A.L. (2009). Attentional tunneling and task management in synthetic vision displays. The International Jour nal of Aviation Psychology, 19, 182-199.

Wilcock, R. and Bull, R. (2014). Improving the performance of older witnesses on identification procedures. In M. Toglia, D. Ross, J. Pozzulo and E. Pica (eds), The elderly eyewitness in court (pp. 118-134). New York: Psychology Press.

Wilcock, R., Bull, R. and Milne, R. (2008). Criminal identification by witnesses: Psychology and practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Wilcock, R., Bull, R. and Vrij, A. (2005). Aiding the performance of older eyewitnesses. Psychiatry, Psychology and Law, 12, 129-141.

Wilding, J., Cook, S. and Davis, J. (2000). Sound familiar? The Psychologist, 13, 558-562.

Wilding, J.M. and Valentine, E.R. (1994). Memory champions. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 231-244.

Williamon, A. and Valentine, E. (2002). The role of retrieval structures in memorizing music. Cognitive Psychology, 44(1), 1-32.

Williams, A.M., Vickers, J. and Rodrigues, S. (2002a). The effects of anxiety on visual search, movement kinematics, and performance in table tennis: a test of Eysenck and Calvo’s processing efficiency theory. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24, 438-455.

Williams, H.L., Conway, M.A. and Cohen, G. (2008). Autobiographical Memory. In G. Cohen and M.A. Conway (eds), Memory in the real world. Hove: Psychology Press.

Williams, J.M.G., Watts, F.N., MacLeod, C. and Mathews, A. (1988). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. New York: Wiley.

Williams, S.J., Wright, D.B. and Freeman, N.H. (2002b). Inhibiting children’s memory of an interactive event: the effectiveness of a cover-up. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6, 651-664.

Williamson, A.M. and Feyer, A.M. (2000). Moderate sleep deprivation produces impairments in cognitive and motor performance equivalent to legally prescribed levels of alcohol intoxication. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 57(10), 649-655.

Williamson, V. (2014). You are the music: How music reveals what it means to be human. London: Icon Books.

Williamson, V.J., Liikkanen, L.A., Jakubowski, K. and Stewart, L. (2014). Sticky tunes: how do people react to involuntary musical imagery? PLOS ONE, 9(1).

Wilmer, J.B., Germine, L., Chabris, C.F., Chatterjee, G., Williams, M., Loken, E., Nakayama, K. and Duchaine, B. (2010). Human face recognition ability is specific and highly heritable. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 5238-5241.

Wilson, M. (2008). From processing efficiency to attentional control: a mechanistic account of the anxiety-performance relationship. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 184-201.

Wilson, M.R. (2012). Anxiety, attention, the brain, the body, and performance. In S. Murphy (ed), The Oxford handbook of sport and performance. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Wilson, M., Smith, N.C. and Holmes, P.S. (2007). The role of effort in influencing the effect of anxiety on performance: testing the conflicting predictions of processing efficiency theory and the conscious processing hypothesis. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 411-428.

Winkler, I., Haden, G.P., Ladinig, O., Sziller, I. and Honing, H. (2009). Newborn infants detect the beat in music Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(7), 2468-2471.

Wister, J.A., Stubbs, M.L. and Shipman, C. (2013). Mentioning menstruation: a stereotype threat that diminishes cognition? Sex Roles, 68, 19-31.

Withington, D. (1999). Localisable alarms. In N.A. Stanton and J. Edworthy (eds), Human factors in auditory warnings. Aldershot: Ashgate.

Wolfe, J.M., Horowitz, T.S. and Kenner, N.M. (2005). Rare items often missed in visual searches. Nature, 435(7041), 439-440.

Woodhead, M.M., Baddeley, A.D. and Simmonds, D.C.V. (1979). On training people to recognize faces. Ergonomics, 22(3), 333-343.

Worchel, P. and Dallenbach, K.M. (1947). ‘Facial vision’: perception of obstacles by the deaf-blind. American Journal of Psychology, 60, 502-553.

World Health Organization. (2011). Mobile phone use: A growing problem of driver distraction. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.

Wright, A. and Alison, L. (2004). Questioning sequences in Canadian police interviews: constructing and confirming the course of events. Psychology, Crime & Law, 10, 137-154.

Wright, D.B., Loftus, E.F. and Hall, M. (2001). Now you see it; now you don’t: inhibiting recall in the recognition of scenes. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 471-482.

Wright, D.B., Memon, A., Skagerberg, E.M. and Gabbert, F. (2009). When eyewitnesses talk. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 174-178.

Wright, K.P., Hull, J.T. and Czeisler, C.A. (2002). Relationship between alertness, performance, and body temperature in humans. American Journal of Physiology-Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 283(6), R1370-R1377.

Wright, P. and Crow, R.A. (1973). Menstrual cycle: effects on sweetness preference in women. Hormones and Behaviour, 4, 387-391.

Wright, R. & Powell, M. (2007). What makes a good investigative interviewer of children? A comparison of police officers’ and experts’ perceptions. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management, 30(1), 21-31.

Wundt, W. (1874). Grundzuge der Physiologischen Psychologie (Principles of physiological psychology). Berlin: Springer.

Yaari, G. and Eisenmann, S. (2011). The hot (invisible) hand: can time sequence patterns of success/failure in sports be modelled as repeated random independent trials? PLoS ONE, 6, e24532.

Yarmey, A.D. (2013). The psychology of speaker identification and earwitness memory. In R.C. Lindsay, D.F. Ross, J.D. Read and M.P. Toglia (eds), The handbook of eyewitness psychology (vol II, pp. 101-136). Hove: Psychology Press.

Yarmey, D. and Morris, S. (1998). The effects of discussion on eyewitness memory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1637-1648.

Yarmey, D., Jacob, J. and Porter, A. (2002). Person recall in field settings. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 2354-2367.

Yarrow, K., Brown, P. and Krakauer, J.W. (2009). Inside the brain of an elite athlete: the neural processes that support high achievement in sports. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 585-597.

Yates, J.F., Veinott, E.S. and Patalano, A.L. (2003). Hard decisions, bad decisions: on decision quality and decision aiding. In S.L. Schneider and J. Shanteau (eds), Emerging perspectives on judgment and decision research (pp. 13-63). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Yechiam, E. and Hochman, G. (2013). Losses as modulators of attention: review and analysis of the unique effects of losses over gains. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 497-518.

Yechiam, E., Rakow, T. and Newell, B.R. (2015). Super-underweighting of rare events with repeated descriptive summaries. Journal of Behavioural Decision Making, 28, 67-75.

Yiend, J. and Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A - Human Experimental Psychology, 54(3), 665-681.

Yiend, J., Mathews, A., Lee, B., Dunn, B., Cusack, R. and Mackintosh, B. (2008). An investigation of the implicit control of the processing of negative pictures. Emotion, 8, 828-837.

Yiend, J., Mathews, A. & Mackintosh, B. (2005). Enduring consequences of experimentally induced biases in interpretation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(6), 779-797.

Yin, R.K. (1969). Looking at upside-down faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 141-145.

Young, A.W., Hay, D.C. and Ellis, A.W. (1985). The faces that launched a thousand slips: everyday difficulties and errors in recognizing people. British Journal of Psychology, 76(4), 495-523.

Young, A.W., Hellawell, D. and Hay, D.C. (1987). Configurational information in face perception. Perception, 16(6), 747-759.

Yuille, J.C. and Cutshall, J. (1986). A case study of eyewitness memory for a crime. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 291-301.

Yuille, J.C. and Cutshall, J. (1989). Analysis of the statements of victims, witnesses and suspects. In J.C. Yuille (ed), Credibility assessment, Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.

Yuille, J.C., Tollestrup, P., Porter, S., Marxsen, D. and Herve, H. (1998). Some effects of marijuana on eye-witness memory. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 20, 1-23.

Zajac, R. (2009). Investigative interviewing in the courtroom: child witnesses under cross-examination. In R. Bull, T. Valentine and T. Williamson (eds), Handbook of the psychology of investigative interviewing (pp. 161-180). Oxford: Wiley Blackwell.

Zajac, R., Gross, J. and Hayne, H. (2003). Asked and answered: questioning children in the courtroom. Psychiatry, Psychology and Law, 10, 199-209.

Zajac, R. and Hayne, H. (2003). I don’t think that’s what really happened: the effect of cross-examination on the accuracy of children’s reports. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 18-195.

Zajac, R. and Hayne, H. (2006). The negative effects of cross-examination style questioning on children’s accuracy. Applied Cognitive Psychol ogy, 20, 3-16.

Zhang, J. and Norman, D.A. (1994). Representations in distributed cognitive tasks. Cognitive Science, 18, 87-122.

Zhu, B., Chen, C., Loftus, E.F., Lin, C., He, Q. and Chen, C. (2010). Individual differences in false memory for misinformation: cognitive factors. Memory, 18, 543-555.

Zinke, K., Zeintl, M., Rose, N.S., Putzmann, J., Pydde, A. and Kliegel, M. (2014). Working memory training and transfer in older adults: effects of age, baseline performance, and training gains. Developmental Psychology, 50, 304-315.

Zsambok, C.E. and Klein, G.A. (eds), Naturalistic decision making (pp. 3-16). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Zsambok, C.E. (1997). Naturalistic decision making: where are we now? In C.E. Zsambok and G.A. Klein (eds), Naturalistic decision making (pp. 3-16). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Zue, V.W. (1985). The use of speech knowledge in automatic speech recognition. Proceedings IEEE, 73, 1062-1615.